Wongetal 2016Self-CompassionWriting
Wongetal 2016Self-CompassionWriting
Wongetal 2016Self-CompassionWriting
Writing Can Heal: Effects of Self-Compassion Writing among Hong Kong Chinese College
Students
Celia C. Y. Wong
Winnie W. S. Mak
Department of Psychology
Author Note
All authors are affiliated with the Department of Psychology at the Chinese University of
Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed
This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the APA journal. It is not
Abstract
Self-compassion has been repeatedly shown to be associated with mental and physical well-
being. Recent studies showed that self-compassion writing can promote mental well-being,
but this has not been examined among Chinese populations. The present study examined the
effectiveness of self-compassion writing among Chinese students. One hundred and twelve
university students were recruited and randomly assigned into one of the following two
writing conditions: self-compassion writing and control writing. Participants were asked to
write according to the instruction for three times in a week and report their levels of positive
and negative affect immediately after writing. Self-reported depressive symptoms and
mindfulness) and emotion regulation capacities (i.e., attention, clarity and repair) were
assessed at baseline and two follow-ups (1-month, 3-month). Results showed the self-
compassion writing group reported more negative affect across the three days of writing than
the control group. No significant group differences were found in depressive symptoms, self-
group reported a significant drop in physical symptoms at the 1-month and 3-month follow-
up, while the control writing group reported no significant change in physical symptoms
across time. The findings suggested that self-compassion writing may benefit physical
health, but further studies should be conducted to examine its underlying mechanism.
Writing Can Heal: Effects of Self-Compassion Writing among Hong Kong Chinese College
Students
compassion is defined as being aware of and having a concern to alleviate both suffering of
the self and others (Goetz, Keltner, & Simon-Thomas, 2010). It is a self-caring attitude in the
concept of “discriminating wisdom” in the Buddhist philosophy that asserts all actions are
perceived inadequacy and disregards the positive or negative aspects of the self (Neff, Hsieh,
& Dejitterat, 2005). Drawing on the writing of various Buddhist teachers (e.g., Salzberg,
1997), Neff (2003a, 2008) defined self-compassion with three components: self-kindness,
common humanity, and mindfulness. Self-kindness refers to being warm and understanding
towards the self when one is suffering or encountering inadequacy or failure. Instead of
passing harsh criticism and judgment on oneself, individuals with self-kindness treat
themselves gently, accept reality with concern and kindness, and experience emotional
shared human experience instead of an isolated experience that happens to one alone. The
last component, mindfulness, refers to maintaining a nonjudgmental and receptive mind state,
putting their aversive situation into a larger perspective, and observing own thoughts and
feelings with openness and clarity at times of adversities and hardships. The mindfulness
on individuals’ state of mind during difficult and aversive situations rather than being a
Self-compassion is similar to but broader than other positive aspects of the self that were
previously studied in literature related to self-esteem and humanistic psychology (Barnard &
Curry, 2011). Even though both self-esteem and self-compassion represent positive attitudes
toward the self, they are not equivalent but only moderately associated (Neff, 2003a). One
major difference is that self-esteem is contingent on social comparison (Tesser, 1999) and
meeting standards (Kernis, 2003) that tends to separate the self from others, while self-
compassion is not contingent on social comparison and it emphasizes the sense of relatedness
instead of separation (Neff & Vonk, 2009). Moreover, research showed self-compassion
could provide and explain additional variance of healthy functioning over and beyond those
explained by self-esteem (Neff & Vonk, 2009), suggesting the two self-concepts are distinct
from each other. Humanistic themes such as “unconditional positive regard” (Rogers, 1961)
accepting personal weakness and adopting an unconditionally caring stance toward oneself,
are largely consistent with the self-kindness dimension of self-compassion. However, self-
incorporating a sense of shared humanity (i.e., common humanity) and a broad and balanced
research has showed its significant and positive associations with physical health (Raque-
Bogdan, Ericson, Jackson, Martin, & Bryan, 2011), life satisfaction, and positive affect, and
negative associations with negative affect, depression, and anxiety (Neff, 2003a; Neff,
Kirkpatrick, & Rude, 2007; Neff & Vonk, 2009). Self-compassion was also found to provide
significant additional variance on well-being above and beyond that provided by social
support (Neely, Schallert, Mohammed, Roberts, & Chen, 2009). Such findings suggested
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 5
that self-compassion may play a distinct role in self-help intervention and promotes health
individuals to bring awareness to their emotions, approach distress with kindness and
understanding, and have a sense of shared humanity. With a more adaptive approach to their
distress, individuals may gradually transform their negative emotions into more positive ones,
have a better understanding of situations, adopt more effective coping strategies to deal with
their stressors (Neff, 2003a, 2003b), and hence experience better outcomes.
Findings from empirical research supported the mediating role of emotion regulation
adaptive emotional processing (Heffernan, Griffin, McNulty, & Fitzpatrick, 2010; Neff,
2003a; Neff et al., 2005), and a study found that emotion regulation mediated the association
between self-compassion and stress (Finlay-Jones, Rees, & Kane, 2015). Experimental data
also showed self-compassion cultivating exercises (e.g., the “Gestalt two-chair” exercise,
compassionate mind training exercise, and mindful self-compassion program) could serve as
reductions in emotional distress, depression and anxiety, and an enhanced ability to soothe
and reassure the self (Diedrich, Grant, Hofmann, Hiller, & Berking, 2014; Gilbert & Procter,
2006; Kelly, Zuroff, & Shapira, 2009; Neff & Gerner, 2013; Neff et al., 2007). All these
findings suggested that emotion regulation may be the mechanism underlying the health
benefits of self-compassion.
Writing Intervention
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 6
effective way to promote well-being among the Chinese by facilitating self-compassion and
emotional regulation capacities. (e.g., Gilbert & Procter, 2006; Kelly et al, 2009; Neff et al.,
2007), which make them less feasible to serve as self-help tools. Expressive writing is a
widely examined self-help intervention (Frattaroli, 2006), which allows individuals a safe
context to acknowledge their feelings, and gradually understand, validate and accept them
(Greenberg & Lepore, 2004; Lepore et al., 2002). Through writing, individuals experience an
increase in emotional intelligence and emotional self-efficacy (Kirk, Schutte, & Hine, 2011),
especially when they are explicitly asked to reflect on emotion regulation in their writing
(Wing, Schutte, & Bryne, 2006). Previous studies showed that expressive writing is
particularly helpful for Chinese and Asian Americans (Lu & Stanton, 2010; Lu, Zheng,
Young, Kagawa-Singer, & Loh, 2012), probably because these ethnic groups have a low
tendency to seek mental health services (Chu, Hsieh, & Tokars, 2011; Nguyen & Lee, 2012)
and preference for self-help intervention over external professional help (Han & Pong, 2015)
due to stigmatization of mental illnesses in lay cultures (Lee, Lei, & Sue, 2000; Mak & Chen,
2010).
among Chinese. So far, only limited studies have examined the benefits of self-compassion
writing (e.g., Baum & Rude, 2013; Imrie & Troop, 2012; Leary et al., 2007; Odou & Brinker,
2014). Among them, only two experimental studies have explicitly induced all three
components of self-compassion in the writing paradigm (Leary et al., 2007; Johnson &
O’Brien, 2013), and none of them were conducted with Chinese or Asian participants. In the
experienced negative event that made them feel bad about themselves (Leary et al., 2007).
Three prompts were given to the self-compassion writing group, with each prompt focused on
Specifically, participants were asked to list ways in which other people also experience
similar events (to promote common humanity), to express understanding, kindness, and
concern to themselves in the same way that they might comfort to a friend who had
undergone the same experience (to promote self-kindness), and to describe their feelings
about the experience in an objective and unemotional fashion (to promote mindfulness).
Results showed that the self-compassion writing group reported significantly lower levels of
post-writing negative affect than the typical expressive writing group. These results
replicated a study by Odou and Brinker (2014), which suggested that guiding individuals to
adopt a self-compassionate attitude soon after a distressing event may provide them timely
relief from negative emotions and hence promote adaptive emotional processing. Later,
Johnson and O’Brien (2013) adopted the writing prompts developed by Leary et al (2007)
and replicated the study to examine the effect of repeated self-compassion writing (three
times a week). They found participants in the self-compassion writing group reported
while no significant changes were reported by the standard expressive writing group or the
control group. These findings suggested that self-compassion writing had a stronger effect in
eliminating the negative mood associated with distressing events and facilitating emotion
Nevertheless, it should be noted that the self-compassion writing paradigm used in these
two studies may not exactly resemble the conceptualizations of self-compassion components
defined by Neff (2003a, 2003b); specifically, the writing prompts seem to over-simplify the
reflecting on how others may experience similar events, but also acknowledges suffering and
failures as a shared part of the human experience. While mindfulness involves a balanced
approach to individuals’ thoughts and feelings, it is not tantamount to processing the events
in an unemotional way, which can lead to emotion suppression. Instead of ignoring the
Although the three self-compassion components were not specified in the writing paradigm
used in their study, Baum and Rude (2013) developed writing instructions adapted from self-
compassion and mindfulness interventions (Leary et al., 2007; Segal, Williams, & Teasdale,
2002) that implicitly capture the concepts of self-kindness (e.g., express the same sort of
kindness, understanding, and compassion towards yourself as you would towards someone
you really care about), common humanity (e.g., remember that part of the human experience
includes experiencing difficult feelings about distressing events) and mindfulness (e.g., try to
bring curiosity to your experience and be accepting of any emotions or thoughts that arise).
Results showed that depression-prone individuals who wrote in this self-compassion writing
paradigm tended to report less depressive symptoms than those in the control condition who
wrote objectively (i.e., without emotion) about how they spent their time, suggesting self-
Neff (2009), the developer of the self-compassion scale, also created a self-compassion
journal exercise with a more comprehensive writing paradigm on the three self-compassion
components than the one used by Baum and Rude (2013). However, as far as research that
can be accessed, this writing paradigm has not been tested in any empirical studies, and thus
writing instruction that could thoroughly illustrate and guide individuals to write on the three
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 9
writing paradigm has never been examined among Asians, who tend to have a higher level of
emotion suppression (Butler, Lee, & Gross, 2007; Soto, Perez, Kim, Lee, & Minnick, 2011)
and a lower level of self-compassion (Neff, Pisitsungkagarn, & Hseih, 2008) than their
The present study attempted to test the effectiveness of self-compassion writing and
its potential driving mechanisms (i.e., self-compassion and emotion regulation). The two
aims of the present study were: (1) to examine the effect of self-compassion writing on post-
writing mood as well as psychological and physical health among Hong Kong Chinese, and
Writing about negative experiences may cause temporary emotional disturbances and
short-term emotional costs, but later health benefits were expected (Pennebaker & Beall,
1986). Therefore, it was hypothesized that group differences would be observed in positive
and negative affect across the three days of writing, but improvement in psychological and
physical health would appear at follow-up assessments. In particular, lower levels of positive
affect and higher levels of negative affect were expected in the self-compassion writing group
than the control writing group immediately after writing, but more reduction of depressive
and physical symptoms for the self-compassion group than the control group at the 1-month
and 3-month follow-ups. Furthermore, in line with the literature and empirical findings that
(Finlay-Jones, Rees, & Kane, 2015; Neff, 2003a, 2003b), we expected that the self-
components and emotion regulation capacities at follow-ups than the control writing group.
regulation capacities would mediate the condition effect on the reducing of depressive and
physical symptoms.
Method
Participants
112 university students were recruited from the XXX University via mass mailing
in two cohorts (one in the spring semester, the other in the summer semester). Data from 47
participants were excluded: 5 dropped out after baseline assessment and did not participate in
the writing intervention, 2 were outliers with multiple scores beyond 3 SD, and 40
participants dropped out at 3-month follow up assessment (20 from the self-compassion
writing group and 20 from the control writing group). No participants dropped out at 1-
month follow-up assessment. As a result, the data of 65 participants (30 men and 35 women;
mean age = 20.5, SD = 1.43) were retained for analysis. There were 33 participants in the
self-compassion writing condition (mean age = 20.70, SD = 1.43; 17 men and 16 women)
and 32 participants in the control writing condition (mean age = 20.31, SD = 1.42; 13 men
and 19 women). Among the participants, 35.6% were freshmen, 26% were sophomores,
20.2% were juniors, 6.8% were senior, and 11.5% were graduate students; the majority were
social sciences (26.9%) and sciences (20.2%) students, followed by arts (15.4%), engineering
(12.5%) and business (11.5%) students. Most students did not have a religion (78.8%), and
Procedures
The present study adopted a mixed research design, with writing condition as the
between subject variable and time of assessments as the within subject variable. The entire
study was implemented on the Internet platform. Participants completed the baseline
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 11
assessment one week before the writing sessions. Then, stratified by gender, participants
were randomly assigned to one of the two conditions: self-compassion writing and control
writing. Both experimenter and participants were blind to the condition assignment.
The first writing session took place in the laboratory. A briefing session was given to
the participants, in which experimenter explained the study procedure and provided a list of
suicide prevention hotlines and contact information of counseling centers. Participants then
entered into an individual room and logged into websites that contained writing instructions
specified for their assigned conditions. Participants were asked to follow the writing
instructions and type in the textbox(es) in their native Chinese language (three designated
textboxes in the self-compassion writing condition, one textbox for each self-compassion
component in the order of mindfulness, common humanity and self-kindness; one designated
textbox in the control writing condition; Andersson & Conley, 2013; Seih, Chung, &
Pennebaker, 2011). Sessions were timed such that a pop-up screen would appear when time
was up, and participants were automatically logged off from the website. A restriction was
also set such that participants could not submit their text until 20 minutes had passed.
Disclosing negative events can be a difficult and upsetting process, which typically results in
short-term distress (Smyth, 1998). Therefore, it is a common practice to assess the short-term
emotional cost of emotional disclosure in expressive writing research (Pennebaker & Beall,
1986; Smyth, 1998). In this study, immediately after the writing session, participants were
asked to complete a brief measurement on mood (i.e., Positive and Negative Affect Schedule,
PANAS). Participants completed their writing and mood assessment in three consecutive
days. After the first laboratory writing session, they completed the second and the third
online writing sessions at home or any private space as they preferred. One month and three
months after the final writing session, participants completed the follow-up assessments. The
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 12
study has been approved by the Survey and Behavioral Research Ethics Committee of XXX,
Self-compassion writing. Participants followed the same writing instruction each day.
Based on the self-compassion journal exercise developed by Neff (2009) and the self-
instruction was created and used in the present study. Participants were asked to write about
a recent event that was painful or they felt bad, or any time that they had judged themselves,
and then to use an accepting and self-compassionate attitude to process the experience.
Three prompts, each centered on the concept of mindfulness, common humanity and self-
Control writing. Previous research suggested that asking control participants to write
on neutral and trivial topics (e.g., description of your living room) may cause a control group
deterioration effect, and that having participants to write about their daily activities in a
factual and unemotional manner maybe a more appropriate control (Radcliffe, Lumley,
Kendall, Stevenson, & Beltran, 2007). As such, control participants in the present study were
asked to write about their daily activities in factual and unemotional manner: Day 1, what
they did over the last week; Day 2, what they did over the last 24 hours; Day 3, what they
Measures
All measures used in this study are Chinese. When available, the Chinese version of
measures was used. Measures lacking an available Chinese version in current literature,
namely the Cohen-Hoberman Inventory of Physical Symptoms (Cohen & Hoberman, 1983)
and the Trait Meta-Mood Scale (Salovey, Mayer, Goldman, Turvey, & Palfai, 1995), were
translated into Chinese using the backward translation method (Brislin, 1970) by two
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 13
independent translators who are bilingual in Chinese and English. Discrepancies were
Mood. The 20-item Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS; Watson, Clark, &
Tellegen, 1988) was used to measure individuals’ levels of positive and negative mood states
immediately after each day of writing to capture participants’ momentary changes of mood
due to writing. Participants rated themselves on a 5-point scale from (1) very slightly or not at
all to (5) very much. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas of positive affect across the
three days of writing ranged from .84 to .90, and Cronbach’s alphas of negative affect across
Scale (CES-D; Irwin, Artin, & Oxman, 1999) was used to measure individuals’ level of
depression. Participants rated themselves on a 4-point scale from (0) rarely or none of the
time to (3) most or all of the time. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas at the baseline and
(CHIPS; Cohen & Hoberman, 1983) was used to measure individuals’ level of common
physical symptoms including sleep problems, constant fatigue, stuffy head or nose, acid
stomach or indigestion and poor appetite. Participants used a 5-point scale ranged from (0)
not at all to (4) extremely to indicate the degree to which the symptom has bothered them in
the past week. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas at the baseline and the follow-up
Self-compassion. The Self-Compassion Scale (SCS; Neff, 2003a) was used to assess
that our interest was to examine how self-compassion writing may promote self-compassion
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 14
qualities that facilitate emotion regulation, only the positive subscales of SCS were analyzed
in the present study. The 13 items assessed individuals’ acts toward themselves at difficult
times; participants indicated how often they behave in the stated manner on a 5-point scale
ranged from (1) almost never to (5) almost always. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas
of the subscales at the baseline and 1-month follow-up assessment ranged from .72 to .87.
Emotion regulation. The 30-item Trait Meta-Mood Scale (Salovey, Mayer, Goldman,
Turvey, & Palfai, 1995) was used to measure individuals’ ability to reflect upon and manage
regulation: (1) attention – the degree of attention devoted to personal feelings; (2) clarity –
the clarity of feelings experienced; and (3) repair – the ability to regulate mood states.
Participants rated themselves on a 5-point scale from (1) strongly disagree to (5) strongly
agree, with higher scores indicating better emotion regulation skills. In the present study,
Cronbach’s alphas of subscales at the baseline and 1-month follow-up assessment ranged
group had engaged in the condition-specific writing instruction, the writing content was
analyzed with the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count program (LIWC2007; Pennebaker,
Chung, Ireland, Gonzales, & Booth, 2007). Related word categories were examined,
including positive and negative emotion words, causation words, and insight words. Text
analysis is a standard procedure to show that emotional and cognitive processing was
Data Analysis
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 15
Analyses were carried out in several stages. First, tests of baseline equivalence and
attrition analysis were carried out. Second, as manipulation check, the self-compassion
writing and the control group were compared on their average use of words across the three
days of writing. Third, the immediate effect of self-compassion writing and control writing
on mood, and their effect on psychological and physical health at follow-ups were examined.
Finally, to explain the potential mechanisms of self-compassion writing, the effect of self-
examined, to evaluate the mediating roles of self-compassion and emotion regulation in the
Results
Baseline Equivalence
The completers in the self-compassion writing group and the control writing group were
square test were conducted. Given that the scores of physical symptoms were positively
skewed (skewness ranged from 1.43 to 2.26), log transformation was applied to the scores,
and the transformed scores were then used in analysis. The two groups did not differ on
demographic information (all ps > .28) or variables of interest (i.e., depressive symptoms,
Attrition Analysis
tests and chi-square test were conducted. The two groups did not differ on demographic
information (ps > .06) and variables of interest at baseline (ps > .09).
Manipulation Check
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 16
The writing content of the self-compassion writing group and the control group was
compared by examining the average percentage use of words across the three days of writing.
word use. The self-compassion writing group wrote significantly more positive emotion
words (e.g., love, nice, sweet), negative emotion words (e.g., hurt, ugly, nasty), causation
words (e.g., because, effect, hence) and insight words (e.g., think, know, consider) than the
time (D1, D2, vs. D3) and condition (self-compassion writing vs. control) were conducted to
examine the between-group differences in positive affect and negative affect immediately
following each writing session. Cohort of participant recruitment (spring semester vs.
summer semester) was added as a covariate in the analysis because previous study showed
that baseline stress would influence the effectiveness of writing intervention (Arigo & Smyth,
2012), and participants recruited in the spring semester may have more academic stressors
and workload than those in the summer semester and thus respond differently to the writing
intervention.
The three way interaction of mood x time x condition was not significant, F (2, 124) =
0.37, p = .69, but a significant interaction effect was found between mood and condition, F
(2, 62) = 9.54, p < .001. Participants in the self-compassion writing condition tended to
report lower levels of positive affect and higher levels of negative affect than the control
up, 3-month follow-up) and condition (self-compassion writing vs. control) was conducted to
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 17
examine the between-group differences in depressive symptoms across time, with the cohort
of participant recruitment controlled. Results showed the interaction effect between time and
condition was not significant, F (2, 124) = 0.94, p = .39 (see Table 3).
3-month follow-up) and condition (self-compassion writing vs. control) was conducted to
examine the between-group differences in physical symptoms across time, with the cohort of
participant recruitment controlled. The interaction effect between time and condition was
marginally significant, F (2, 113.09) = 2.81, p = .07,η2p = .04. Specifically, the interaction
effect between time and condition was significant at 3-month follow-up, F (1, 62) = 4.03, p <
.05,η2p = .06, but not significant at 1-month follow-up, F (1, 62) = 3.03, p = .09, η2p = .05. To
follow-up the interaction effect between time and condition, repeated measure ANOVA was
conducted in each condition separately. A significant main effect of time was found in the
self-compassion writing condition, F (2, 30) = 4.89, p < .05, η2p = 0.25. Participants reported
a significant drop in physical symptoms from baseline to 1-month follow-up, F(1, 31) = 9.88,
p < .01, η2p = .24, and 3-month follow-up, F(1, 31) = 6.86, p < .05, η2p = .18. However, the
time effect was not significant in the control writing condition, F (2, 29) = 1.43, p = .26.
follow-up, F(1, 30) = 0.81, p = .38, and 3-month follow-up, F(1, 30) = 2.91, p = .10 (see
Table 3).
To determine whether the participant drop out at 3-month follow-up may contaminate
the findings, a repeated measure ANOVA by time (baseline vs. 1-month follow-up) and
condition (self-compassion writing vs. control) was also conducted with baseline and 1-
month data only (including data from participants who dropped out at the 3-month follow-up
assessment). Similar to the findings with 3-month data, results showed the interaction effects
between time and condition on depressive symptoms, F (1, 102) = 0.00, p = .95, and physical
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 18
symptoms, F (1, 102) = 1.39, p = .24, were not significant, but the trend of reduced physical
symptoms in the self-compassion writing group at 1-month follow-up, F(1, 51) = 10.61, p
< .01, was similar to the findings with 3-month data that participants in the self-compassion
3-month follow-up) and condition (self-compassion writing vs. control) was conducted to
common humanity, and mindfulness) across time, with the cohort of participant recruitment
controlled. Results showed the interaction effect between time and condition on self-
compassion components was not significant, F (6, 252) = 0.41, p = .87 (see Table 3).
follow-up, 3-month follow-up) and condition (self-compassion writing vs. control) was
attention, clarity, and repair) across time, with the cohort of participant recruitment
controlled. Results showed the interaction effect between time and condition on emotion
regulation capacity was not significant, F (6, 248) = 0.52, p = .79 (see Table 3).
To examine whether the participant drop out at the 3-month follow-up assessment may
change the findings, repeated measure MNOVA by time and condition was also conducted
with baseline and 1-month data only. Similar to the findings with 3-month data, results
showed the interaction effects between time and condition on self-compassion components, F
(3, 101) = 0.20, p = .90, and emotion regulation capacities, F (3, 101) = 0.57, p = .64, were
components and emotion regulation capacities at the 1-month and 3-month follow-ups was
shown in the self-compassion writing group, no further analysis was carried out to examine
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 19
Discussion
The present study extended previous research by examining the effectiveness of self-
compassion writing among Chinese. It was expected that the self-compassion writing
paradigm could facilitate self-compassion and emotion regulation, which in turn lead to
health benefits.
The findings of the present study were in line with the hypotheses and previous research
that expressive writing resulted in emotional cost immediately after writing but health
benefits at later follow-ups (Pennebaker & Beall, 1986). The self-compassion writing group
reported significantly higher level of post-writing negative affect than the control writing
month follow-up assessments was reported in the self-compassion writing group, while no
change in physical symptoms was reported in the control writing group. Although no
writing group or the control writing group, these results were consistent with previous meta-
analysis results that expressive writing has a weak impact on depression (r = .04, p < .05;
Frattaroli, 2006). Also, in line with previous research which suggested that Asians tend not
to report affective complaints because of the stigmatization of mental illness in Asian cultures
(for a review, see Lee, Lei, & Sue, 2000; Mak & Chen, 2010), the mean score of baseline
depressive symptoms in the present study was very low (approximately 10 out of 30). The
low score suggested that the majority of the participants reported they did not have frequent
depressive symptoms (“some or little of the time,” or “1-2 days during the week,” referring to
the anchors of the CES-D Scale) before the writing intervention. Hence, there was a potential
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 20
floor effect, which made reduction in depressive symptoms by self-compassion writing less
feasible.
2003b; Odou & Brinker, 2014); therefore, the present study examined self-compassion and
inconsistent with the hypothesis, the results showed no significant improvement in self-
if not long-term. On the other hand, other research suggested that self-compassion is a
process that requires more time to occur (Shahar et al., 2012), and writing once each week
over a month may be more effective than writing four times within a single week
(Pennebaker, 1997). It is possible that 3-day self-compassion writing may not be sufficient in
writing practice is required. Moreover, it is possible that the small changes in self-
compassion and emotion regulation capacities may be better captured with more sensitive
measured, such as the Self-Compassion and Self-Criticism Scales (Falconer, King, & Brewin,
2015), the Assessing Emotions Scale (Schutte et al., 1998) or the Emotional Self-Efficacy
Scale (Kirk, Schutte, & Hine, 2008). It is also possible that self-compassion writing
promotes physical health with alternative mechanisms, such as reducing self-criticism and
promoting adaptive coping strategies, and further research is needed to examine this
speculation.
There were several limitations in the present study. First, due to convenience sampling,
only Hong Kong Chinese college students were recruited in the present study. The writing
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 21
task may elicit events that widely vary in psychological significance across individuals.
Meta-analysis found that a larger effect of expressive writing was revealed in community
samples and samples with existing physical health problems or trauma or stress history than
in university student samples (Frattaroli, 2006), future studies should extend the present study
to a greater diversity of samples, especially those who have adjustment difficulties or distress.
Second, some researchers suggested that having the ability to regulate emotions is not
equivalent to actual utilization of emotion regulation ability when facing problems, and
emotional self-efficacy may better reflect actual utilization of emotion regulation than trait
emotional intelligence (Kirk et al., 2011). Only the measurement of emotional intelligence
was included in the present study, which may limit our understanding of emotion regulation
benefit health through enhancing individuals’ efficacy in regulating emotions. Lastly, the
present study did not examine possible moderators of self-compassion writing. It was
suggested that people with fear of emotions, impaired affect tolerance, and alexithymia may
benefit from emotion disclosure more than others (Greenberg & Lepore, 2004). The health
benefits of self-compassion writing may thus be more pronounced among these individuals.
Notwithstanding these limitations, the findings of the present study are inspiring. It is one of
the first attempts to investigate the health benefits and mechanisms of self-compassion
Conclusion
The present study was one of the few studies which attempted to examine the potential
Americans and further research is needed to support this argument. However, not requiring
the general public, especially for populations who do not have access to professional care
services (Kazdin & Blase, 2011), and those reluctant to seek mental health services, such as
With the prevalent cultural belief that any psychological problem is a result of
personality weakness or a lack of self-control and willpower (Kung, 2004), and the resulting
stigmatization of mental health problems in Asian societies, Asians and Asian Americans
tend not to express their emotional problems, and they find expression of psychological
distress through the body (i.e., somatization) more acceptable (Lee, Lei, & Sue, 2000; Mak &
Chen, 2010). Instead of seeking mental health services to deal with their problems, they
would prefer using self-help methods (Han & Pong, 2015). Self-compassion writing, a self-
help method that allows individuals to receive intervention (i.e., write about their distressing
experience) at their personal time and space, and without the disclosure of personal identity
and hence potential risk of stigmatization, may be an alternative approach to reach out to
References
Andersson, M. A., & Conley, C. S. (2013). Optimizing the perceived benefits and health
outcomes of writing about traumatic life events. Stress and Health, 29, 40-49.
Arigo, D., & Smyth, J. M. (2012). The benefits of expressive writing on sleep difficulty and
appearance concern for college women. Psychology and Health, 27, 210-226.
symptoms in participants with low initial depression. Cognitive Therapy & Research,
37, 35-42.
Bennett-Goleman, T. (2001). Emotional alchemy: How the mind can heal the heart. New
Brislin, R. (1970). Back translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross Cultural
psychology, 1, 185-216.
Butler, E. A., Lee, T. L., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion regulation and culture: Are the social
Chu, J. P., Hsieh, K., & Tokars, D. A. (2011). Help-seeking tendencies in Asian Americans
with suicidal ideation and attempts. Asian American Journal of Psychology, 2, 25-38.
Cohen, S., & Hoberman, H. M. (1983). Positive events and social supports as buffers of life
Diedrich, A., Grant, M., Hofmann, S. G., Hiller, W., & Berking, M. (2014). Self-compassion
Ellis, A. (1973). Humanistic psychotherapy: The rational-emotive approach. New York, NY:
Julian Press.
SELF-COMPASSION WRITING 24
Falconer, C. J., King, J. A., & Brewin, C. R. (2015). Demonstrating mood repair with a
Finlay-Jones, A. L., Rees, C. S., & Kane, R. T. (2015). Self-compassion, emotion regulation
and stress among Australian psychologists: Testing an emotion regulation model of self-
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0133481
Gilbert, P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high shame
and self-criticism: Overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach. Clinical
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis
Goldstein, J., & Kornfield, J. (1987). Seeking the heart of wisdom: The path of insight
Han, M. & Pong, H. (2015). Mental health help-seeking behaviors among Asian American
community college students: The effect of stigma, cultural barriers, and acculturation.
and emotional intelligence in nurses. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 16, 366-
373.
Imrie, S., & Troop, N. A. (2012). A pilot study on the effects and feasibility of compassion-
focused expressive writing in Day Hospice patients. Palliative & Supportive Care, 10,
115-122.
Irwin, M., Artin, K. H., & Oxman, M. N. (1999). Screening for depression in the older adult:
Criterion validity of the 10-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale
Kazdin, A. E., & Blase, S. L. (2011). Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice to
Kelly, A. C., Zuroff, D. C., & Shapira, L. B. (2009). Soothing oneself and resisting self-
Kirk, B. A., Schutte, N. S., & Hine, D. W. (2011). The effect of an expressive-writing
Kirk, B. A., Schutte, N. S., & Hine, D. W. (2008). Development and preliminary validation
Kung, W. (2004). Cultural and practical barriers to seeking mental health treatment for
Leary, M. R., Tate, E. B., Adams, C. E., Allen, A. B., & Hancock, J. (2007). Self-compassion
Lee, J., Lei, A., & Sue, S. (2000). The current state of mental health research on Asian
Lepore, S. J., Greenberg, M. A., Bruno, M., & Smyth, J. M. (2002). Expressive writing and
Lepore & J. M. Smyth (Eds.), The writing cure: How expressive writing promotes health
Association.
Lu, Q., & Stanton, A. L. (2010). How benefits of expressive writing vary as a function of
Lu, Q., Zheng, D. H., Young, L., Kagawa-Singer, M., & Loh, A. (2012). A pilot study of
Mak, W. W. S., & Chen, S. X. (2010). Illness behaviors among the Chinese. In M. H. Bond
(Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Chinese Psychology, 2nd Ed. (pp. 421-439). Oxford:
Neely, M. E., Schallert, D. L., Mohammed, S. S., Roberts, R. M., & Chen, Y. (2009). Self-
kindness when facing stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation, and support in
compassion.org/
Association.
Neff, K. D., & Dahm, K. A. (in press). Self-compassion: What is is, what it does, and how it
Neff, K. D., & Gerner, C. K. (2013). A pilot study and randomized controlled trial of the
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y., & Dejitterat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, achievement goals, and
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and adaptive
Neff, K. D., Pisitsungkagarn, K., & Hseih, Y. (2008). Self-compassion and self-construal in
the United States, Thailand, and Taiwan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39,
267-285.
Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self esteem: Two different
Nguyen, D., & Lee, R. (2012). Asian immigrants’ mental health service use: An application
Odou, N., & Brinker, J. (2014). Exploring the relationship between rumination, self-
Pennebaker, J. W., Chung, C. K., Ireland, M., Gonzales, A., & Booth, R. J. (2007). The
Radcliffe, A. M., Lumley, M. A., Kendall, J., Stevenson, J. K., & Beltran, J. (2007). Written
emotional disclosure: Testing whether social disclosure matters. Journal of Social and
Raque-Bogdan, T. L., Ericson, S. K., Jackson, J., Martin, H. M., & Bryan, N. A. (2011).
Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S. L., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. P. (1995). Emotional
attention, clarity, and repair: Exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-
Mood Scale. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure, & health (pp. 125-154).
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., &
Shahar, B., Carlin, E. R., Engle, D. E., Hegde, J., Szepsenwol, O., & Arkowitz, H. (2012). A
Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression: Effect sizes, outcome types, and
Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive
comparison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion, 11, 1450-
1455.
Baumeister (Ed.), The self in social psychology (pp.446-460). New York: Psychology
Press.
Warner, L. J., Lumley, M. A., Casey, R. J., Pierantoni, W., Salazar, R., Zoratti, E. M., …
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief
measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality
Wing, J. F., Schutte, N. S., & Bryne, B. (2006). The effect of positive writing on emotional