Week 1 Foundations of Effective Reading
Week 1 Foundations of Effective Reading
Week 1 Foundations of Effective Reading
The focus on language development is particularly important when the language demands of
early schooling are high as when pupils are learning multiple language as in the Philippines.
(Snow, Burns & Griffin, 1998). The implementation of MTB-MLE puts a high demand on pupils’
ability to understand and speak MT, Filipino, and English with proficiency by Grade 3.
At an early age, children begin to develop an understanding of print and how it works.
Knowledge about print is gained through many interactions with language—both oral and
written. Through these experiences, children come to understand that written language is
related to spoken language and print carries a message (meaning). By the time children begin
school, many are able to “read” environmental print; however, they may not understand how
print conveys meaning (Bialystok, Shenfield, & Codd, 2000). This understanding is foundational
for learning to read and predicts later reading achievement. Skills to be developed are:
Students who are beginning to read should be able to demonstrate the following print
knowledge skills (Snow et al., 1998):
Phonological awareness is the ability to attend to and manipulate units of sound in speech
(syllables, phonemes) independent of meaning (Yopp & Yopp, 2009). Phonological awareness is
complex and critical when learning to read. Students should be able to:
When students have built phonological awareness, they have the foundational skills needed for
using the alphabetic principle (Troia, 2004).
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
Proficient readers demonstrate skill in both comprehension and word identification (Snow et al.,
2005). This ability to accurately and rapidly decode words and then associate the printed word
with its meaning allows the reader to focus attention on the meaning of the text.
Alphabetic Principle: Knowledge of the shapes and names of letters of the letters
and the understanding that there is a systematic relationship between the sounds of
spoken languages (MT, Filipino, English) and the letters and letter patterns of written
languages.
Decoding and Phonics: Understanding how to read each letter or letter pattern
in a word to determine the word’s pronunciation and meaning.
Irregular/High-Frequency Words: Recognition of words that appear often in
printed English but may not be readily decodable.
Sometimes used synonymously with the term decoding, phonics has been defined as “the ability
to associate letters and letter combinations with sound and blending them into syllables and
words” (What Works Clearinghouse, 2007, p. 4). Experts have described phonics instruction as a
method of teaching children to learn and use the alphabetic principle (Armbruster et al., 2001).
In order to blend the sounds together to pronounce words, students must learn progressively
difficult word patterns—individual letter sounds progress to letter combinations and continue to
include structural analysis such as prefixes and suffixes. Research confirms that systematic and
explicit phonics instruction yields positive results for developing readers (Chall, 1967; Foorman,
Fletcher, & Francis, 1998).
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
Fluency has been defined as “the ability to read a text quickly, accurately, and with proper
expression” (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000, pp. 3-5).
As skillful readers develop, their recognition skills move from simple patterns to more
complicated ones, and they transition from understanding simple letter patterns to mastering
patterns of language structure (Meyer & Rose, 1999). Quick and accurate word identification is
essential for skillful reading and allows cognition to shift to comprehending the text, yet it is just
one facet of comprehension (Torgesen et al., 2007).
For this reason, some researchers have adopted the visual image of fluency as the bridge
connecting word decoding and comprehension (Rasinski, 2004). This “bridge” is composed of a
range of skills a reader must build and maintain in order to become and remain fluent (Kuhn &
Stahl, 2003):
Research has established that children’s vocabularies, or “knowledge of words and word
meanings” are crucial to their acquisition of reading skills and their understanding of what they
read. In fact, “one of the most enduring findings in reading research is the extent to which
students’ vocabulary knowledge relates to their reading comprehension (Lehr, Osborn, &
Hiebert, 2004).
Studies indicate that students add approximately 2,000 to 3,500 distinct words to their reading
vocabularies every year (Anderson & Nagy, 1992; Beck & McKeown, 1991). Several principles
regarding vocabulary development are important to keep in mind (Nagy & Scott, 2000):
Word knowledge is complex: knowing a word is more than being able to recite its
definition.
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
Vocabulary development and word knowledge are tremendously complex, and this is due, in
part, to the many types of vocabularies, and the social and academic language, students must
develop (Hiebert, 2008).
Researchers concur that reading comprehension is very complex. Fluency, while important,
does not ensure good reading comprehension and is just one of several proficiencies that a
skilled reader must possess (Paris, Carpenter, Hamilton, & Hamilton, 2005; Torgesen et al.,
2007). Proficient readers are able to comprehend both narrative and expository texts. In order
to become skilled readers by the end of elementary school, students must be able to do the
following (Lehr, Osborn, & Hiebert, 2004, p. 5)
Comprehension is a process that involves strategic action throughout the reading process:
before, during, and after reading. Before reading, the student sets a purpose for reading, makes
predictions or formulates questions. During reading, predictions may be confirmed and
questions answered; however, the student may also reformulate or add questions and
predictions. A “good comprehender” also self-regulates by monitoring understanding, clarifying
confusions, and repairing comprehension. After reading, the student reflects on the reading and
summarizes or synthesizes the new information. Knowledge-building, active reading for specific
purposes and applying the new knowledge gained deepen comprehension.
Researchers have identified three types of comprehension processing (Block, Rodgers, &
Johnson, 2004):
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
Readers who employ the third form of processing, metacognitive, are strategic. They are able to
control their reading and employ a variety of strategies to ensure that they understand what
they read. These strategies are “actions that are consciously controlled by a reader but may be
applied unconsciously when the reading materials are easy or when the reader is skilled in using
a specific strategy” (Blachowicz & Ogle, 2008).
Moving beyond the findings of the National Reading Panel (2000) researchers have found that
the following list of strategies “have been researched and validated to be highly successful since
2000” (Block & Duffy, 2008, p. 22):
Students’ comprehension depends on: (1) how well they read the words on the page; (2) how
much knowledge they possess; (3) how well they think; and (4) how motivated they are to do
the work (Torgesen, 2005). Snow, Burns, & Griffin (1998) confirm that motivation is one
predictor of reading success. Simply defined, motivation is the desire to do something. In the
context of reading development, doing something might be reading a picture book, participating in
a discussion, or learning about photosynthesis (Gaskins, 2003).
At an early age, positive expectations about and positive experiences with literacy foster
motivation to read. While most children begin school with a positive attitude and expect to be
successful, those who have negative experiences with reading and struggle to master
components decide that they do not like to read and they do not want to read (Juel, 1988). For
some children, this happens as early as the first grade.
Viewed in this light, it is of paramount importance for children to have positive experiences and
to develop and maintain the motivation to read (Leipzig, 2001). A motivated reader has the
following characteristics:
Appreciates the pleasures of reading
Views reading as a social act
Uses reading as an opportunity to explore interests
Reads widely for a variety of purposes
Works comfortably with a variety of written forms and genres of text
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
REFERENCES
Anderson, R. C., & Nagy, W. E. (1992). The vocabulary conundrum. American Educator,
16(Winter), 14-18, 44-47.
Armbruster, B. B., Lehr, F., & Osborn, J. (2001). Put reading first: The research building blocks
for teaching children to read. Kindergarten through grade 3. Washington, DC: National
Institute for Literacy.
Bialystok, E., Shenfield, T., & Codd, J. (2000). Languages, scripts, and the environment: Factors in
developing concepts of print. Developmental Psychology 36(1), 66-76.
Blachowicz, C., & Ogle, D. (2008). Reading comprehension: Strategies for independent learners (2nd
ed.). New York: Guilford.
Block, C. C., Rodgers, L. L., & Johnson, R. B. (2004). Comprehension process instruction: Creating
reading success in grades K-3. New York: Guilford.
Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Ehri, L. C. (2002). Phases of acquisition in learning to read words and implications for teaching.
British Journal of Educational Psychology: Monograph Series, 1, 7-28.
Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Francis, D. J. (1998). Preventing reading failure by ensuring
effective reading instruction. Retrieved March 15, 2003, from
http://www.usu.edu.Teachall/text/reading/ReadingFailure
Gaskins, I. W. (2003). Taking charge of reader, text, activity, and context variables. In A. P.
Sweet & C. E. Snow (Eds.), Rethinking reading comprehension. Solving problems in the
teaching of literacy (pp. 141-163). New York: Guilford.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American
children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Hiebert, E. H. (2008, October 6). Reading content-area texts: What's involved for English
language learners. Paper presented at the CREATE conference, Minneapolis, MN.
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first grade
through fourth grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437-447.
Kuhn, M. R., & Rasinski, T. (2007). Best practices in fluency instruction. In L. B. Gambrell, L. M.
Morrow & M. Pressley (Eds.), Best practices in literacy instruction (3rd ed., pp. 204-219).
New York: Guilford.
Lehr, F., Osborn, J., & Hiebert, E. H. (2004). Research-based practices in early reading series: A
focus on vocabulary. Honolulu, HI: Pacific Resources for Education and Learning.
Retrieved March 17, 2009, from
http://www.eric.ed.gov/ERICWebPortal/contentdelivery/servlet/ERICServlet?accno=ED4
83190
Meyer, A., & Rose, D. H. (1999). Learning to read in the computer age. Cambridge, MA: Brookline
Books.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National
Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific
research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH
Publication No. 00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the
scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Reports of
the subgroups. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Public
Health Service, National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development.
Paris, S. G., Carpenter, R. D., Hamilton, A. H., & Hamilton, E. E. (2005). Spurious and genuine
correlates of children's reading comprehension. In S. G. Paris & S. A. Stahl (Eds.),
Children's reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 131-160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Snow, C. E., Burns, S. M., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children.
Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Effective Literacy Instruction for New K–3 Teachers
Snow, C. E., Griffin, P., & Burns, M. S. (Eds.). (2005). Knowledge to support the teaching of reading:
Preparing teachers for a changing world. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Torgesen, J., Houston, D., Rissman, L., & Kosanovich, M. (2007). Teaching all students to read in
elementary school: A guide for principals. Portsmouth, NH: RMC Research
Corporation, Center on Instruction. Retrieved January 13, 2009, from
http://www.centeroninstruction.org/files/Principals%20Guide%20Elementary.pdf
Torgesen, J. K. (2005). Dyslexia, and other things that make it difficult to learn to read
proficiently (PowerPoints). Keynote presentation at meetings of the Utah Branch of the
International Dyslexia Association, Salt Lake City, UT (March). Retrieved December 23,
2008, from http://www.fcrr.org/science/pdf/torgenson/Utah_keynote.pdf
What Works Clearinghouse. (2007). Beginning reading. Retrieved March 11, 2009, from
http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/reports/beginning_reading/topic/
Yopp, R. H., & Yopp, H. K. (2000). Supporting phonemic awareness development in the
classroom. Reading Teacher, 54(2), 130-143.