Krashen's Monitor Model Assignment

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7

Assignment No.

:
(1)

Class:
BS-ENG-VIII

Subject:
Applied Linguistics
Topic:
“Krashen’s Monitor Model”

Submitted to:
Madam Qudsia Ghouri
Submitted by:
Asad Mahmood
Roll No. 05
Session 2017-21
KRASHEN'S MONITOR MODEL OF SLA
In the 1970s, an influential view of relationship between acquisition and
learning was propounded by the American linguist Stephen Krashen. This is a brief
description of Krashen's widely known and well accepted theory of second language
acquisition (SLA), which has had a large impact in all areas of second language
research and teaching since the 1980s. The Monitor Model of Krashen is probably
the most widely cited theory of second language acquisition and has often
dominated education debate in this field. Krashen's Monitor Model has been popular
and influential because of its comprehensive nature and it moves from theory to
classroom practice. It is comprised of five central hypotheses (1983):

1. The Acquisition-Learning Hypothesis


2. The Monitor Hypothesis
3. The Natural Order Hypothesis
4. The Input Hypothesis
5. The Affective Filter Hypothesis

1. The Acquisition-Learning Hypothesis


According to Krashen, language is acquired, not learnt. The acquisition-
learning distinction is the most fundamental of all the hypotheses in Krashen's
theory. There are two different ways of developing skills in a second language:
learning and acquisition. Learning is a conscious Process that focuses the
students' attention on the form of '1' language (structure). Acquisition, unlike
learning, is a process similar to that by which we acquired our mother tongue, and
which represents the subconscious activity by which we internalize the new
language, putting emphasis on message (meaning) rather than on the form.

The first hypothesis of Krashen’s Monitor Model, the acquisition-learning


hypothesis, distinguishes between the processes of language acquisition and
language learning. Krashen contrasts acquisition and learning as two distinct and
separate language processes. Acquisition occurs passively and unconsciously
through implicit, informal, or natural learning, resulting in implicit knowledge and
acquired competence of a language; in other words, to acquire a language is to “pick
up” a language by relying on “feelings” of correctness rather than conscious
knowledge of language rules.

In contrast to acquisition, learning occurs actively and consciously through


explicit or formal learning and instruction, resulting in explicit knowledge about a
language; learning results in metalinguistic knowledge and awareness. Furthermore,
the acquisition-learning hypothesis states that both children and adults acquire
language via access to an innate language acquisition device (LAD) regardless of
age as well as that learning cannot become acquisition. The most important
pedagogical implication of the first hypothesis of the Monitor Model is that explicit
teaching and learning is unnecessary, indeed inadequate, for second language
acquisition.

According to Krashen there are two independent systems of second language


performance: the „acquired system' and 'the learned system'. The 'acquired
system' or 'acquisition' is the product of a subconscious process very similar to the
process children undergo when they acquire their first language. It requires
meaningful interaction in the target language and natural communication in which
speakers are concentrated not in the form of their utterances, but in the
communicative act. The 'learned system' or 'learning' is the product of formal
'instruction and it comprises a conscious process which results in conscious
knowledge 'about' the language, for example knowledge of grammar rules.
According to Krashen 'learning' is less important than 'acquisition'.

Criticism of Acquisition-Learning Hypothesis:


The first critique of Krashen’s Monitor Model is that the hypothesized
distinction between acquisition and learning as posited by the acquisition-learning
hypothesis, or, more specifically, determining whether the process involved in
language production resulted from implicit acquisition or explicit learning, is
impossible to prove. As Barry McLaughlin offers as anecdotal evidence, he feels that
the German *Ich habe nicht das Kind gesehen “I have not seen the children” is
incorrect based on intuition but also knows that the utterance is incorrect based on
his knowledge of the rules of German grammar.

Furthermore, critics consider the argument that learning cannot become


acquisition questionable. Kevin R. Gregg offers anecdotal evidence of his personal
experience learning a second language as counterevidence to the clear division
between acquisition and learning: He initially consciously learned the conjugations of
Japanese verbs through rote memorization, which ultimately led to unconscious
acquisition. In his case, learning became acquisition. Both examples of personal
experience with a second language illustrate the problem with stringently
distinguishing the process of language acquisition from the process of language
learning. Thus, the claim that acquisition is distinct from learning fails to withstand
evidence-based criticism

Although influential within the field of second language acquisition over the
past few decades, the Monitor Model is not without criticism as illustrated by the
major critiques of the learning-acquisition hypothesis.

In most classrooms, learning is more emphasized than acquisition. We have


been teaching grammar rule or rules of usage instead of facilitating acquisition of
English in the classroom. However, in real life, when we take interest with speakers
of our own language, we rarely focus our attention on the form of the speaker uses.
Acquisition is, thus, untutored or naturalistic way. Acquiring a language is more
successful and longer lasting than learning.

2. The Monitor Hypothesis


Krashen's SLA theory was originally known as Monitor theory, perhaps
because the central part of it was the Monitor Hypothesis. The monitor hypothesis
explains the relationship between acquisition and learning and defines the influence
of the latter on the former. The monitoring function is the practical result of learned
grammar. According to Krashen, the acquisition system is the utterance initiator,
while the learning system performs the role of the monitor or the editor. The Monitor
is an editing device that may operate before language performance. The monitor
acts in a planning, editing and correcting function when three specific conditions are
met: that is the second language learner has sufficient time at his/her disposal,
he/she focuses on form or thinks about correctness, and he/she knows the rule.
This hypothesis takes into consideration three kinds of Monitor users:
i. Over-monitor users, i.e., students who are seldom trust their
acquired competence, thus verifying every sentence they produce by
using their learned competence. Such speakers are sure to speak
hesitantly and with no fluency (introvert). Lack of self-confidence is
frequently related to the over-use of the 'monitor'.
ii. Under-monitor users; i.e., speakers who do not really care
about correctness, only meaning. These speakers are usually very
talkative (extrovert) even in their own mother tongue, and, although
they make more mistakes than over-monitor users, they will also
covey more meaning.
iii. Optimal monitor users, or acquirers who manage to use
Monitor only when it is appropriate. Optimal monitor users usually
give the impression that they possess more competence tan under
monitor users of the same level of acquisition.

The monitor hypothesis, complements the acquisition-learning hypothesis by


claiming that the only function of learning within second language acquisition is as an
editor, or Monitor, for language use produced by the acquired system as well as to
produce grammatical forms not yet acquired. The Monitor allows a language user to
alter the form of an utterance either prior to production by consciously applying
learned rules or after production via self-correction. In other words, the learned
system monitors the output of the acquired system.

However, according to the monitor hypothesis, explicit knowledge of a


language rule is not sufficient for the utilization of the Monitor; a language user must
also have an adequate amount of time to consciously think about and apply learned
rules. Additionally, the three conditions required by the Monitor—time, focus, and
knowledge—are, as Krashen asserts, “necessary and not sufficient,” meaning that,
despite the convenement of all three conditions, a language user may not utilize the
Monitor.

Criticism of the Monitor Hypothesis


The major critique of the monitor hypothesis expands on the critique of the
acquisition-learning hypothesis. According to the monitor hypothesis, the main
purpose of language learning is to function as a Monitor for output produced by
acquired system. However, as critics reveal through deeper investigation of the
acquisition-learning distinction, to separate language learning clearly and adequately
from language acquisition is impossible. Consequently, determining that the function
of the learned system is as a Monitor only remains likewise impossible to prove.

Additionally, that the claim of learning-as-Monitor applies only to output after


production invites further criticism of the hypothesis; second language learners can
and do use the learned system to produce output as well as to facilitate
comprehension. Such questions and evidence, therefore, invalidate the central claim
of the monitor hypothesis.

Therefore, in spite of the influence of the Monitor Model in the field of second
language acquisition, the third hypothesis, the monitor hypothesis, has not been
without criticism as evidenced by the critiques offered by other linguists and
educators in the field.

3. The Natural Order Hypothesis


The natural order hypothesis is based on research findings that the
acquisition of grammatical structures follows a 'natural order' which is predictable.
For a given language, some grammatical structures tend to be acquired early while
others late. For instance, one of the structural items that we teach in most language
programmes is the third Person singular of the simple present tense. Surprisingly,
this is one of the structures acquired last. This is why we often complain of having
intermediate, or even advanced, students who make mistakes with such a simple
pattern.

The Natural Order Hypothesis also accounts for students‟ mistakes and
errors: Students make mistakes (or developmental errors) when the structure used
has not been completely acquired. We can assume that mistakes will be present
during the acquisition process.

Consequently, the best way to correct the students' mistakes is to provide


more input containing the structure in question

Criticism of the Natural Order Hypothesis


The second critique of the Monitor Model surrounds the evidence in support of
the natural order hypothesis. According to Krashen, that children acquiring English
as a second language acquire the morphemes of the language in a predictable
sequence similar but not identical to the sequence followed by children acquiring
English as a first language confirms the validity of the natural order hypothesis.
Furthermore, other morpheme studies on adults acquiring English as a second
language show similar results.

However, as Kevin R. Gregg argues, to generalize the results of a study on


the acquisition of a limited set of English morphemes to second language acquisition
as a whole is fallible. Morpheme studies offer no indication that second language
learners similarly acquire other linguistic features (phonology, syntax, semantics,
pragmatics) in any predictable sequence let alone in any sequence at all.

Secondly, the natural order hypothesis fails to account for the considerable
influence of the first language on the acquisition of a second language; in fact, the
results of other studies indicate that second language learners acquire a second
language in different orders depending on their native language. Therefore, although
posited by the natural order hypothesis, second language learners do not
necessarily acquire grammatical structures in a predictable sequence.

Although the Monitor Model has been influential in the field of second
language acquisition, the second hypothesis, the natural order hypothesis, has not
been without criticism as evidenced by the critiques offered by other linguists and
educators in the field.
4. The Input Hypothesis
The input hypothesis is Krashen's attempt to explain how the learner acquires
a second language. So, the Input hypothesis is only concerned with 'acquisition’ not
'learning'. Krashen's input hypothesis claims that language acquisition occurs
through understanding messages or through receiving 'comprehensible input'.
That is, in Krashen's view, perceptive language behaviours such as listening or
reading play the major role in the learning process, including the development of
speaking ability or the knowledge of grammar rules will follow automatically as long
as a sufficient amount and type of input is provided.

According to this hypothesis, the learner improves and progresses along the
'natural order' when he/she receives second language 'input' that is one step
beyond his/her current stage of linguistic competence. For example, if a learner is at
a stage 'i', then acquisition takes place when he/she is exposed to 'Comprehensible
Input' that belongs to level 'i + 1’. Since not all of the learners can be at the same
level of linguistic competence at the same time, Krashen suggests that natural
communicative input is the key to designing a syllabus, ensuring in this way that
each learner will receive some 1+1' input that is appropriate for his/her current stage
of linguistic competence.

Criticism of the Input Hypothesis:


Like for the acquisition-learning hypothesis, the first critique of the input
hypothesis surrounds the lack of a clear definition of comprehensible input; Krashen
never sufficiently explains the values of i or i+1. As Gass et al. argue, the vagueness
of the term means that i+1 could equal “one token, two tokens, 777 tokens”; in other
words, sufficient comprehensible input could embody any quantity.
More importantly, the input hypothesis focuses solely on comprehensible
input as necessary, although not sufficient, for second language acquisition to the
neglect of any possible importance of output. The output hypothesis as proposed by
Merrill Swain seeks to rectify the assumed inadequacies of the input hypothesis by
positing that language acquisition and learning may also occur through the
production of language. According to Swain who attempts to hypothesize a loop
between input and output, output allows second language learners to identify gaps in
their linguistic knowledge and subsequently attend to relevant input. Therefore,
without minimizing the importance of input, the output hypothesis complements and
addresses the insufficiencies of the input hypothesis by addressing the importance of
the production of language for second language acquisition.
Thus, despite the influence of the Monitor Model in the field of second
language learning and acquisition, the input hypothesis, the fourth hypothesis of the
theory, has not been without criticism as evidenced by the critiques offered by other
linguists and educators in the field.

5. The Affective Filter Hypothesis


Understanding a message is not enough to assure language acquisition; one
must be open to the message so that it reaches the Language Acquisition Device
(LAD). where statements are generated. Not all the input reaches the (LAD);
somewhere along the way it is filtered, and only part of it is acquired. This filtering
process takes place in the affective filter, which acts like the gate controlling the
amount of input
The affective filter "opens" or "closes" to our mood that is, if we are relaxed
and in a pleasant learning environment, more input will reach the LAD, while if we
are tense or are in a negative environment, our efforts to provide input is fruitless.
That is why, it is important to provide an appropriate acquisition environment in the
class room, eliminating anxiety and encouraging students so they feel they can really
acquire the language. Of course, proper motivation is the best way to open the
filters. The Affective Filter hypothesis influences the rate of development in second
language learning and the level of success in becoming bilingual.
Stephen Krashen's theory of language learning has been the source of
considerable controversy and academic discussion, but it has undoubtedly
succeeded in bridging the gap between linguistic theory and actual language
teaching by affecting the thinking and attitudes of many practicing teachers.

Criticism of the Affective Filter Hypothesis


The final critique of Krashen’s Monitor Model questions the claim of the
affective filter hypothesis that affective factors alone account for individual variation
in second language acquisition. First, Krashen claims that children lack the affective
filter that causes most adult second language learners to never completely master
their second language. Such a claim fails to withstand scrutiny because children also
experience differences in non-linguistic variables such as motivation, self-
confidence, and anxiety that supposedly account for child-adult differences in second
language learning.

Furthermore, evidence in the form of adult second language learners who


acquire a second language to a native-like competence except for a single
grammatical feature problematizes the claim that an affective filter prevents
comprehensible input from reaching the language acquisition device. As Manmay
Zafar asks, “How does the filter determine which parts of language are to be
screened in/out?” In other words, the affective filter hypothesis fails to answer the
most important question about affect alone accounting for individual variation in
second language acquisition.

Although the Monitor Model has been influential in the field of second
language acquisition, the fifth and final hypothesis, the affective filter hypothesis, has
not been without criticism as evidenced by the critiques offered by other linguists and
educators in the field.

Conclusion:
For many of us, Krashen's Second Language Acquisition theory has changed
our concept of language teaching and has suggested new ideas for communicative
language teaching. The model has been criticized by some linguists and isn't
considered a valid hypothesis for some. It has however, inspired much research, and
many praise its value. The theory underlies Krashen and Terrell's comprehension-
based language learning methodology known as natural approach (1983) .The Focal
Skills approach, first developed in 1998, is also based on the theory.

The End

You might also like