A Study of English Reading Strategies Used by Senior Middle School Students
A Study of English Reading Strategies Used by Senior Middle School Students
A Study of English Reading Strategies Used by Senior Middle School Students
Fenfang Li
School of Foreign Languages, Chongqing Yangtze Normal University
Chongqing 408001, China
E-mail: [email protected]
Abstract
For the past several decades, reading strategies have aroused many researchers’ interests, but the research has
mainly focused on strategy use while ignoring the function of metacognitive awareness of reading strategies. This
study, based on a questionnaire, investigated the students’ awareness of reading strategy use at the senior middle
school level. The major findings are, first, that there is a moderate awareness of all the strategies; secondly, the
students hold a preference for Problem Solving Reading Strategies, followed by Global and Support Reading
Strategies; thirdly, females show higher use of reading strategies than males in each individual category, as well as
in the combined sub-categories. Also, the females are more careful and considerate while the males are more
adventurous and bolder. Finally, the readers’ metacognitive awareness of reading strategies is closely linked to
their language proficiency. Based on the findings above, several suggestions can be made to help teachers improve
their teaching and help the students improve their reading efficiency.
Keywords: Reading, Reading strategies, Metacognition
1. Introduction
Since the 1970s, how students learn a second language has attracted great emphasis. Considerable interest has
been paid to understanding the characteristics of language learners and the learning strategies they use to achieve
success in language learning. In the second language learning, reading serves as the primary source of new
information about all sorts of topics.
In a review of the developments in second language reading research, Grabe (1991) points out that the crucial
importance of the reading skill in academic contexts had led to considerable research on reading in a second
language. The goal of most second language reading programs is to turn “learning to read” into “reading to learn”
(Carrell, 1989).
Traditionally many psychologists and teachers have insisted that reading is nothing more than decoding written
symbols to sounds (i, e, figuring out what the painted word says). The reader was seen as the “recipient” of
information or as an "empty vessel" that brought nothing to the text. This motion of “text as object” is now
frequently discredited in reading circles because readers are not entirely passive. In recent years, an increasing
number of researchers believe that reading process is an active process in which reading skills can be developed.
The viewpoint has moved from the “text as object” to that of the “text as process” by encouraging close interaction
between the reader and the text. In addition to model-making in second language reading during the two decades,
considerable attention has been paid to understanding what proficient, skilled readers typically do while reading,
including identifying the strategies they use and how and under what conditions they use those strategies. This line
of research has been useful in instructing non-proficient first and second language readers to increase their
awareness and use of reading strategies to improve comprehension (Garner, 1985)
Many previous studies recognized the role of metacognitive awareness in reading comprehension, whether one is
reading in the native language or a second language. Indeed, the consensus view is that strategic awareness and
monitoring of the comprehension process are critically important aspects of skilled reading. Many researchers
have applied metacognitive theory to reading in recent years. Researchers generally agree that metacognition is
one of the most important elements to influence reading comprehension. O'Malley et al (1985) finds that
intermediate ESL students use more metacognition than beginners. They thought students of higher proficiency in
target language could even better use metacognition to control their learning than beginning students. Collins et al
(2003) have done a research synthesis to examine recent research on the relation between metacognition and
reading comprehension based on reviews and primary researches since 1985 and 1986.
Although there is an explosion of research related to the relationship between metacognitive awareness and
reading proficiency, it is easy for researchers to ignore the two factors: First, in China most research on
metacognitive awareness is measured by different instruments: Oxford’s SILL and Mokhtari and Reichard’s
MARSI, making it difficult to compare the results from two different instruments; Secondly, the instruments have
been widely used in western countries, but in China, the potentially influential variables such as the learners’
cultural background, personality, learning style, motivation, attitude and so on are significantly different. As a
result, the other results may not be consistent with that in China.
2. Literature Review
2.1 Reading
2.1.1 Definition of Reading
Broadly speaking, there are two types of reading. Reading can occur on two different levels: Reading may mean
looking at a written text in order to understand its contents. This is the first type of reading. It is usually done
silently. The understanding that results is called reading comprehension. The second type of reading refers to
speaking or reciting a written text aloud (oral reading). This can be done with or without an understanding of the
contents. In this article, the concept of reading refers only to reading with comprehension, although, we sometimes
do use oral reading to demonstrate that we comprehend.
In the past two decades, the concept of reading has come into the field of ELT (English Language Teaching) and
gained much favor among language researchers and practitioners as well. Alderson (1984), Clarke and Coady
(1979) and Silberstein (1994:12) have proposed different definition of reading from different perspectives. To
sum up, reading is conceptualized as an interactive cognitive process in which readers interact with the text using
their prior knowledge, cultural background and use appropriate strategies.
2.2 Reading Strategies
2.2.1 Definition of Reading Strategies
Drawing on works in cognitive psychology, strategies are defined as learning techniques, behaviors,
problem-solving or study skills which make learning more effective and efficient (Oxford & Crookall, 1989). In
the context of reading, reading strategies indicate how readers conceive a task, what textual cues they attend to,
how they make sense of what they read, and what they do when they do not understand (Block, 1986). Reading
strategies refer to "the mental operations involved when readers purposefully approach a text and make sense of
what they read" (Barnett, 1988)
In short, reading strategies are deliberate, conscious techniques that readers employ to enhance their
comprehension or retention of the textual information. Specifically they have the following
characteristics:(1)deliberate, conscious plans, techniques and skills; (2) aiming to enhance reading
comprehension and overcome comprehension failures; and (3) behavioral and mental. They are of interest for
what they reveal about the way readers manage their interaction with the written text and how these strategies
are related to text comprehension (Carrell, 1989).
2.2.2 Sub-categories of Reading Strategies
A lot of research has been done on how proficient readers employ strategies while reading. Based on various
criteria, reading researchers usually divide reading strategies differently. In this article, Mokhtari and Reichard’s
Reading Strategy Model was employed.
Mokhtari and Reichard were famous for the MARSI (the metacognitive awareness of reading strategies
inventory. It covers three sub-categories: Global Reading Strategies, Problem-Solving Reading Strategies and
Support Reading Strategies.
Global Reading Strategies contain 13 items and represent a set of reading strategies oriented toward a global
analysis of text. Examples include “evaluating what to read or ignore, noting text characteristics, guessing what
the material is about, etc.” These strategies can be thought of as generalized, intentional reading strategies aimed
at setting the stage for the reading act.
The second subcategory (Problem-Solving Strategies ) contain 8 items that appear to be oriented around
strategies for solving problems when the test becomes difficult to read. Examples of these strategies include
re-reading for better understanding, going back when losing concentration, pausing and thinking about reading
etc. These strategies provide readers with action plans that allow them to navigate through the text skillfully.
Such strategies are localized, focused problem- solving or repair strategies used when problems develop in
understanding textual information.
The third subcategory (Support Reading Strategies) contains 9 items and primarily involves use of outside
reference materials, taking notes, underlining or circling information and other practical strategies that might be
described as functional or support strategies. These strategies provide the support mechanisms aimed at
sustaining responses to reading.
2.3 Metacognition
2.3.1 Definition of Metacognition
Metacognition, or thinking about thinking, is formally defined as “any knowledge or cognition that takes as its
object, or that regulates any aspect of any cognitive endeavor” (Flavell, 1981). Metacognition includes
knowledge about learning and about oneself as a learner, and the skills of monitoring and regulating one's own
cognitive processes. In his 1976 article, Flavell first used the term metacognition formally in the title of his paper.
He defined metacognition as follows: “In any kind of cognitive transaction with the human or non-human
environment, a variety of information processing activities may go on. Metacognition refers, among other things,
to the active monitoring and consequent regulation and orchestration of these processes in relation to the
cognitive objects or data on which they bear, usually in the service of some concrete goal or objective." (p.232).
To further illustrate the constructs of the metaconition, Anderson (2001) offered us a figure of metacognition as
follows:
Insert Figure 1 Here
Based on the previous research, metacognition can be seen as an individual’s knowledge of their own cognitive
process and their ability to independently control these processes by organizing, monitoring and modifying them
as a function of learning.
2.3.2 Metacognitive Awareness and Reading Comprehension
Metacognitive awareness or knowledge is knowledge about ourselves, the tasks we face, and the strategies we
employ (Baker and Brown, 1984). Knowledge about ourselves may include knowledge about how well we
perform on certain types of tasks or our proficiency levels. Knowledge about tasks may include knowledge about
task difficulty level. For example, in the area of reading, we may know that familiar-topic material is easier to
understand than unfamiliar material; explicit sentences assist us in tasks that require reduction of texts to their gists.
About strategies, we may know that verbal rehearsal and elaboration of material assist in retrieval, or that
prediction of article content based on titles improves comprehension, and so forth. Metacognitive awareness
therefore, also involves the awareness of whether or not comprehension is occurring, and the conscious application
of one or more strategies to correct comprehension (Baumann, Jones, anf Seifert-Kessel, 1993). This body of work
has enormous explanatory power for description of the reading process in both the L1 and L2 contexts. First
language reading researchers, most notably Baker and Brown (1984) have investigated several different aspects of
the relationship between metacognitive ability and effective reading. Two dimensions of metacognitive ability
have been recognized: 1) knowledge of cognition or metacognitive awareness; and 2) regulation of cognition
which includes the reader's knowledge about his or her own cognitive resources, and the compatibility between the
reader and the reading situation. For example, if a reader is aware of what is needed to perform effectively, then it
is possible to take steps to meet the demands of a reading situation more effectively. If, however, the reader is not
aware of his or her own limitations as a reader or of the complexity of the task at hand, then the reader can hardly
be expected to take actions to anticipate or recover from difficulties (Carrell, 1989).
Research on metacognition in reading was carried since the late 1970s. It takes into account not just the product of
comprehension (how readers perform on given comprehension tasks), but also the cognitive processes involved
while reading which are typically not able to be detected through traditional reading tests. It is now generally
agreed that the relationship between language proficiency and reading ability is reciprocal; for example, Alderson
(1984) and Cziko(1978) have suggested that high levels of overall competence in the target language often lead to
improvement in reading ability in that language. On the other hand, other researchers (e. g. Carrell et al., 1989)
provide evidence that proficient L2 readers can compensate for a lack of English proficiency by increasing
awareness and usage of reading strategies while reading to enhance comprehension.
Another important facet, the difference between males and females in reading strategy use, catches the researcher’s
attention. Young and Oxford (1997) have found that gender can have a significant impact on students’ strategy use.
An emerging theory for this gender difference proposes that although sometimes males surpassed females in the
use of a particular strategy, females employ more strategies or employ strategies more effectively (Erhman and
measure for a student’s language proficiency). There are six parts to the test: Listening comprehension (30 marks),
multiple choice (15 marks), close test (30 marks), reading comprehension (40 marks), dialogue completion (10
marks) and writing (25 marks) with the total marks 150. The students’ scores were used as an indicator of their
English proficiency.
3.4 Data Collection and Analysis
On May 4, 2007, the MARSI instrument was administered at the beginning of each individual class, with the help
of the classroom teacher. The students were informed of the purpose of the survey and of the fact that there were no
right or wrong answers. They were asked to complete the survey in 8-10 minutes and to express their authentic
opinions for each statement. Each completed survey was manually examined, and, after discarding the invalid ones,
the 180 usable questionnaires were collected.
After that, each questionnaire was examined and coded for statistical analysis to answer the research questions
listed above. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 13.0) was employed for the statistical
analysis of the data and the significance level of p. < 0.05 was set. The analysis covered the following three points:
1) Use of descriptive statistics to work out the mean scores and standard deviation of the overall reading strategies
and each strategy category.2) Use of independent t-tests to examine whether there were any differences in the use
of reading strategies between the high-proficiency and low-proficiency students. 3) Use of independent t-tests to
examine whether significant differences existed between the males and females with respect to reported strategy
use while reading.
4. Results and Discussion
4.1 Research Question 1
What type and frequency of reading strategies do the students use in their reading process ?
The means of the three sub-categories are presented in the table.
Insert Table 1 Here
Of the three strategy sub-categories, Problem Solving Reading Strategies average scores were highest (3.22),
followed by Global Reading Strategies (2.97) and Support Reading Strategies (2.60). In other words, when
problems in reading arise, most of these students are ready to adopt strategies like reading slowly and carefully to
be sure, going back when losing concentration, rereading for better understanding and so on to solve problems. By
contrast, they use far fewer Support Reading Strategies like reading aloud when the test gets hard, discussing to
check understanding, asking oneself questions and so on. This result coincides with the findings in Kouider
Mokhatari and Carla Reichard’s (2002) and Monos’s (2004) study in which the Problem Solving Problems
Strategies fall into the High Usage Group, and Support Reading Strategies are the least frequently used among the
three subcategories.
For these students, 5 of the 30 strategies (17%) fell in the High Usage Group (mean of 3.5 of above ), while 18 had
means between 2.50 and 3.50 and the remaining 7 strategies (23%) fell into the Low Usage Group (mean values
below 2.4). According to Table 4.1.1, one can also find the 5 most often and least often used strategies. The 5
highest means are PROB1, PROB2, PROB7, SUP5, GLOB11. The five least often used strategies are GLOB4,
GLOB9, SUP4 and SUP2. Interestingly, Global Reading Strategies and Support Reading Strategies are among
both the most and least often used items. Three Problem Solving Strategies are in the most often used category, but
none is among the least often used ones. Here the five most often and least often used strategies particularly
attracted my attention: Some studies (Kouider Mokhtari and Carla A. Reichard, 2002; Sheorey R. and Mokhtari K,
2001) have covered the same research, but their results are quite different. One possible explanation to that there
may be something to do with different culture, society, environment, teachers, personality, etc.
In Ji’an County Middle School, the students clearly respect their teachers. The teachers are absolute authorities and
the students tend to be obedient rather than critical. When the students are young, they are taught to respect and
obey the teachers absolutely. Questioning or challenging teachers would be seen as rebellious or rude behavior.
Thus under these circumstances, the students accept everything from the teachers without any doubt. However, as
a result, the students’ critical awareness is slowly strangled or killed. Consequently, we can see the frequency of
critically analysing information and asking oneself questions is very low. In addition, Chinese people cherish
managing by oneself in preference to co-operation. People want to do better than others. In this school, the
phenomenon is even more pronounced. That is why the students seldom co-operate with their peers, and don’t
often use the strategy of “discussing to check understanding”.
metacognitive awareness of their own use of strategies, which in turn leads to greater reading ability and
proficiency (Baker and Brown, 1984; Garner, 1987; Pressley and Afflerbach, 1995). Research in first language
literacy also has established a similar correlation between reading ability and strategy use among native speakers
of English. For example, Baker and Brown (1984) and Kletzien (1991) found that poor readers are generally
deficient in reading skills and using strategies. Skilled readers, on the other hand, are able to reflect on and monitor
their cognitive processes while reading. In other words, they know which strategies to use and how to use them,
and they know the conditions under which they ought to be used. More recently, Alexander and Jetton (2000) and
Pressley, M., and Afflerbach, P (2000) have also suggested that awareness of using reading strategies is a
characteristic of superior reading comprehension and successful learning.
We believe that it is important for all readers, proficient and non-proficient, to be aware of the significance of
strategies. Teachers can play a key role in increasing students’ awareness of such strategies and in helping them
become “constructively responsive readers” (Pressley and Afflerbach, 2000). We realize, of course, that an
awareness of strategic reading does not necessarily translate into actual use of the strategies concerned.
Nevertheless, we believe that it is important for metacognitive reading strategies instruction to be integrated into
the overall reading curriculum so as to enhance students’ metacognition with regard to reading. Such instruction
can help promote an increased awareness of the mental processes involved in reading and the development of
thoughtful and constructively responsive reading.
5. Conclusion, Implications and Limitations
5.1 Conclusion
In this study, 180 participants completed a 30-item questionnaire of metacognitive awareness of reading strategies
when reading academic materials. Some findings are consistent with previous research, while some are different.
These findings are summarized below:
1. Of all the three strategy sub-categories, Problem Solving Reading Strategies are the students’ favorite, followed
by Global Reading Strategies and then Support Reading Strategies, irrespective of their reading ability or gender.
2. The students show medium strategy use while reading (mean of Overall Reading Strategies = 2.93)
3. Females show greater awareness of reading strategies in all three sub-categories as well as 24 individual reading
strategies.
4. The high-proficiency students show more frequent use in all the three sub-categories and 27 individual reading
strategies than low-proficiency students.
5.2 Implications
Based on the findings above, several suggestions can be made to help teachers understand more about their
students and to take actions to help students improve their reading.
Cultivating Co-operative Awareness
Using Different Methods to Treat Different Students
Reorienting Teachers’ Roles
5.3 Limitations
Despite the interesting findings and their beneficial implications presented in this study, the research has certain
limitations as follows:
1. The questionnaire used in this study consists of only 30 items. It is obviously not enough to cover all the
strategies used by the students. This, to a certain extent, affects its reliability and the validity of the data in the study.
So a wider variety of assessment methods is recommended in future studies e.g. interviews, observation, verbal
report and diaries.
2. The questionnaire MARSI was widely used in western countries, but in China, the potentially influential
variables such as the learner’s cultural background, personality, learning style, motivation, attitude and so on are
quite different. As a result, the data yielded in the study might cause discrepancies compared with other MARSI
study results. It is then recommended that future researchers design a questionnaire which may better suit Chinese
students.
3. In this study, 137 males and 43 females were involved. Perhaps the uneven number between male and female
students may more or less influence the results. In the future study, more classes can be involved so that more girl
students can finish the questionnaire to make the results more reliable.
References
Alderson, J. C. (1984). Reading in a foreign language: a reading problem or a language problem? In: Alderson,
C., Urquhart, X. (Eds.) Reading in a Foreign Language. Longman, New York, 1-24.
Anderson, J. C. (2001). New Strategies for the Improvement of Automobile Catalysts. International Journal of
Molecular Sciences, 251.
Aebersold, J. and Field, M. L. (1997). From reader to reading teacher: Issues and strategies for second
language classrooms. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
Anderson, N. (1999). Exploring Second Language Reading. Issues and Strategies. Boston: Heinle & Heinle
Publishers.
Baker, L. (1985). Metacognitive skills and reading. In R. Bara, M. L. Kamil, P.Mosenthal, and P. D. Pearson
(Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2). White Plains, NY: Longman, 353-364.
Baker, L. and Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In P. David Pearson (Ed.), Handbook of
reading research. New York: Longman.
Barnett, M. (1988). More than meets the eye. Englewood Cliffs, N. J: Prentice Hall Regents, p66.
Baumann, J, Jones, L and Seifert-Kessel. (1993). Effect of Think Aloud Instruction on Elementary Students'
Comprehension Monitoring Abilities.
Block, E. (1986). The comprehension strategies of second language readers. Tesol Quaterly, 20:3, 463-494.
Carrell, P. L. (1989). Metacognitive awareness and second language reading. Modern Language Journal, 73,
121-134.
Carrell, P. et al. (1988). Interactive Approaches to Second Language Reading. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Clarke, M. and Coady, J. (1979). Reading in Spanish and English. Evidence from Adult ESL Students in
Language Learning, 29, 121-150.
Cziko, G.. (1978). Differences in first and second language reading: the use of syntactic, semantic, and discourse
constraints. Canadian Modern Language Review, 34, 473-489.
Ehrman, M. and Oxford, R. (1990). Effects of sex differences, career choice, and psychological type on adult
language learning strategies. The Modern Language Journal, 73, 1-13.
Flavell, J. H. (1981). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. Resnick (Ed) The nature of intelligence.
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 37-232.
Garner, R. (1985). Metacognition and Reading Comprehension. Ablex Publishing, Norwood, NJ.
Green, J. and Oxford, R. (1995). A Closer Look at Learning Strategies, L2 Proficiency, and Gender. TESOL
Quarterly, (29): 261-297.
O'Malley, J. M., Chamot, A.U., et al. (1994). Learning Strategies Used by Beginning and Intermediate ESL
Students. Language Learning, (35): 21-46.
Oxford, R. (1994). Teaching culture in the language classroom: Toward a new philosophy. In J. E. Alatis (Ed.),
Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics. Washington: Georgetown University Press,
26-45.
Oxford, R., & Crookall, D. (1989). Research on Language learning strategies: methods, language findings, and
instructional issues. Modern Language Journal, 73, 404-419.
Oxford R. and Nyikos, M. (1989). Reflections on sex differences in use of language learning stratigies. Foreign
Language Annals, 21, 296.
Oxford, R. & Crookall, D. (1989). Research on six situational language learning strategies: Methods, findings,
and instructional issues. Modern Language Journal, 73(4).
Paris, S. G.. (1984). Coaching children to use reading strategies. Invited address at the National Reading
Conference, St. Petersburg, Florida. November.
Rao, Z. H. (2005). Gender, Academic Major, and Chinese Students' Use of Language Learning Strategies: Social
and Educational Perspectives. THE JOURNAL OP ASIA TEFL, 2:3, Mokhtari and Reichard. 115-138.
Sheorey, R. (1999). An examination of language learning strategy use in the setting of an indigenized variety of
Table 2. Strategic Differences in the Use of Reading Strategies between Male and Female Students
Male(137) Female(43) t p-value
Name strategy
M S.D. M S.D
GRS Global Reading Strategies 2.92 0.50 3.14 0.49 -2.54 0.012
SRS Support Reading Strategies 2.55 0.52 2.76 0.43 -2.44 0.016
PRS Problem Solving Reading Strategies 3.15 0.58 3.44 0.55 -2.90 0.004
ORS Overall Reading Strategies 2.87 0.44 3.11 0.41 -3.16 0.002