A Model of Loneliness in Older Adults
A Model of Loneliness in Older Adults
A Model of Loneliness in Older Adults
Loneliness and physical health status in older adults have been correlated strongly but the predictive direction is unclear. This
study examined the relationship between personality, cognition, social network, and age modeled as predictors of loneliness
in older Americans. Self-assessed health mediated the relationship. The sample consisted of 208 independently living individ-
uals 60 to 106 years of age from the southern region of the United States. Model comparison revealed health did not mediate
the relationship significantly but that self-reported loneliness itself mediated between personal characteristics and perceived
health. Results indicate anxiety, frequency of telephone contact, and age, but notfrequency of face-to-face contact with
others or cognitive functioning, affect perceived loneliness. Perceived loneliness mediates the effects of anxiety, frequency of
telephone contact, and age on self-assessed health. Feelings of loneliness decrease one's evaluation of physical well-being.
P231
P232 FEESETAL
dently mobile or are separated from family and friends by great past few weeks did you ever feel very lonely or remote from
distances. Visiting suggested some level of independent mobil- other people," with four values ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4
ity, in which case there may be less reliance on the telephone (often). This item was part of the negative affect balance scale
than for less able-bodied persons or for those who live farther with a reported test-retest reliability of .81 (Bradburn, 1969).
from family and friends. Additionally, deterioration in hearing The final measure was a subscale score from the Philadelphia
may actually discourage telephone communication and increase Geriatric Center Morale Scale (PGC; Lawton, 1975) labeled as
reliance on visiting, if possible. Each question, therefore, be- "lonely dissatisfaction." This subscale was composed of six di-
came a single-item indicator reflective of separate constructs; chotomously scored items from which an aggregated subscore
high scores reflected frequent interaction. Age, in years, was was derived. Items represented "the extent to which an individ-
self-reported. ual feels lonely and dissatisfied with life" (Sauer & Warland,
Two indicators from the OARS (Fillenbaum, 1988) served as 1982, p. 223). Cronbach's alpha was reported as .85 (Lawton,
indicators for the latent construct Physical Illness. The first 1975) for the six-item scale. Measures were recoded so that a
measure assessed current perceived overall health, "How would high score reflected greater loneliness.
you rate your overall health at the present time," from 3 (excel- Reliability of the measures within the model may be esti-
lent) to 0 (poor). The second indicator was a comparative indi- mated from the factor loadings; that is, the factor loading is a
cator of health, "Is your health now better, about the same, or measure of the validity of a construct. Because the square root
worse than it was five years ago," from 2 (better) to 0 (worse). of reliability is its validity, validity squared is an estimate of the
Measures were recoded so that high scores reflected illness. reliability of the measure (Bollen, 1989). The presence of error
Reported reliability for the items was .74 (Fillenbaum, 1988). is calculated as part of model estimation. Refer to Table 2 for
Loneliness was measured with three indicators. The first, estimated reliabilities within the model.
"Do you find yourself feeling lonely quite often, sometimes, or
almost never" (OARS; Fillenbaum, 1988), was coded as 0 Data Analysis
(often), 1 (sometimes), and 2 (seldom). The item was a part of A covariance matrix was generated using a listwise proce-
the Affective dimension of social support, with a reported relia- dure (see Table 2). Data were analyzed using the structural
bility of .71. The second measure was taken from the Bradburn equation modeling procedures in LISREL VIII (Joreskog &
Affect Balance Scale (BABS; Bradburn, 1969): "During the Sbrbom, 1994) with maximum likelihood estimation.
A MODEL OF LONELINESS P235
Table 2. Correlation Matrix, Means and Standard Deviations for Measurement Model (n = 208)
Indicator 1 10 11 12 13 14
1. Emotional stability 1.00
2. Apprehension -.42 1.00
3. Tension -.39 .46 1.00
4. Picture arrangement .10 -.11 .02 1.00
5. Block design .16 -.14 .03 .64 1.00
6. Arithmetic .17 -.23 .02 .58 .64 1.00
Table 3. Measurement Model: Standardized Factor Loadings Table 4. Correlations Among Latent Variables
Latent Construct and Indicators Factor Loading Estimated Reliability Latent Variable
Personality 1. Physical Illness —
Emotional stability -.64 .41 2. Loneliness -.55 —
Apprehension .66 .44 3. Anxiety .38 -.50 —
Tension .62 .38 4. Cognition -.40 .24 -.19 —
5. Visiting -.11 .09 .04 .12 —
Cognition
6. Telephone Contact -.19 .31 -.03 .27 .31 —
Picture arrangement .76 .58
Block design 7. Age .27 -.26 -.10 -.61 -.04 -.24 —
.84 .71
Arithmetic .75 .56 Note: n = 208
Visiting Friends/Relatives 1.00 1.00
Telephone Contact 1.00 1.00
pared. First, a nonmediated model was tested in which the direct
Age 1.00 1.00 path coefficient from the mediator to the outcome was fixed to
Physical Health zero and all other paths were estimated (Baron & Kenny, 1986).
Self-health rating .78 .61 Second, a fully recursive, mediated model was run to assess me-
Health problems .37 .14 diation. Total effects were decomposed in the later model.
Loneliness
Feeling lonely (OARS) .60 .36 Model A: Loneliness as Outcome
Feel very lonely/remote (BABS) .74 .55 The focus of thisfirstset of analyses was to examine the direct
Lonely dissatisfaction (PGC) .67 .45 and mediated effects of each construct on loneliness. Results of
"Validity (factor loading) squared is an estimate of the reliability of the indi- the nonmediated model indicated a reasonable fit of the model to
cator (Bollen, 1989). x2 (60) = 88.11,/? < .05. the data, x2 (60) = 88.11,/? < .05, GFI = .95, AGFI = .90.
Regression coefficients were significant between Anxiety and
Physical Illness (standardized coefficients reported), 7 = .48, t =
RESULTS 4.16, as well as Anxiety and Loneliness, 7 = .60, t = 4.57. Higher
An advantage of structural equation modeling was the use of levels of anxiety were related to higher levels of physical illness
multiple indicators of latent constructs. Multiple indicators re- and higher levels of loneliness. Age significantly predicted
flected the specific domain of content defined by the latent vari- Physical Illness, 7 = .23, t = 2.03, and Loneliness, 7 = .36, t =
able, allowed estimation of measurement error, and simultane- 3.16, suggesting the older the individual, the higher the evaluation
ous estimation of parameters in the model. Preliminary of physical illness and the greater loneliness experienced.
confirmatory factor analyses were conducted using LISREL Telephone contact negatively predicted Loneliness, 7 = -.23, t =
VIII (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1994) to estimate the measurement 2.73. Cognition and Visiting were not predictive of either criterion
model with significant indicators of the latent constructs. The variable.
proposed measurement model and regression model were con- According to Baron and Kenny (1986), mediation is present
ducted concurrently (see Table 3 for factor loadings and Table 4 if the significant coefficients between the latent exogenous
for correlations among latent variables). (Anxiety and Age) and the latent endogenous (Loneliness) are
To examine the direct, indirect, and total effects of the latent reduced (partially mediated) or become nonsignificant (fully
exogenous constructs on loneliness, nested models were com- mediated) when the mediating path is present and significant in
P236 FEESETAL.
Table 5. Model A: Decomposition of Effects of Personal Table 6. Model B: Decomposition of Effects of Personal
Characteristics on Loneliness Characteristics on Physical Illness
Note: n = 208. Standardized regression coefficients shown. Note: n - 208. Standardized regression coefficients shown.
*p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.
the model (in this analysis, the direct path between Physical Effects were decomposed in the mediated model (see Table
Ulness and Loneliness). The mediated model, x2 (59) = 76.66, p 5). The total effects of Anxiety, Telephone Contact, and Age on
>.05, GFI = .95, AGFT = .91, was a significant improvement in Loneliness were significant. However, although the direct path
fit over the nonmediated model, A \ 2 (1)= 11.45, p < .001 (see coefficient from Anxiety to Physical Illness was significant, the
Figure 3). However, the mediational path between Physical indirect effect on Loneliness was not (t = 1.81). No significant
Illness and Loneliness barely reached significance (t = 1.97). indirect effects were found.
Several coefficients changed as a consequence of mediational
path estimation. A reduction in the standardized regression coef- Model B: Loneliness as Mediator
ficient between Anxiety and Loneliness from .60 to .42 was ob- The second set of analyses reversed the direction of the arrow
served, which remained significant (t = 3.20). The coefficient between Loneliness and Physical Illness, making Loneliness
from Anxiety to Physical Illness decreased slightly to .48 from the mediating variable. As expected, path coefficients and the fit
.35 and also remained significant (t = 3.45). The path coefficient of the nonmediated model were identical to that of Model A, \ 2
was reduced to nonsignificance between Age and Illness (t = (60) = 88.11, p < .05, GFI = .95, AGFI = .90.
1.44), and the coefficient between Age and Loneliness remained The fit of the mediated model was, again, identical to the fit
significant (t = 2.59). The path coefficient between Telephone of Model A's mediated version; however, the mediational path
Contact and Loneliness dropped slightly (.23 to .21) but re- in Model B was highly significant (standardized beta = .44, t =
mained significant. The effects of Cognition and Visiting on 3.31; see Figure 4). When compared to the nonmediated model,
Loneliness were unaffected by the addition of the mediational the standardized coefficient between Anxiety and Illness was
path. The mediated model accounted for 28% of the variance in reduced to nonsignificance, y = .11, t = .89. The coefficient be-
Physical Illness and 50% of the variance in Loneliness. tween Anxiety and Loneliness decreased only slightly from .60
A MODEL OFLONEUNESS P237
to .55 (t = 4.34). The path from Age to Illness dropped from .23 a significant total effect on feelings of loneliness; the older the
to .00 and was not significant (t = .02). Age to Loneliness (t = participants were, the more likely they were to report loneli-
3.00) and Telephone Contact to Loneliness (t = 2.78) remained ness. Our results also suggest the effect of old age on serf-eval-
virtually unchanged. Cognition emerged as a negative predictor uation of health is effectively mediated by loneliness. With ad-
of Physical Illness, 7 = -.28, t = 2.41. Visiting was not predic- vancing age, feelings of loneliness affect perceptions of health
tive. The mediated model accounted for 40% of the variance in rather than self-perceived health affecting feelings of loneliness.
Loneliness and 40% of the variance in Physical Illness. Health status does appear to decline with age: centenarians
Total effect of Anxiety on Physical Illness was significant (t = reported proportionally more "poor" and "fair" health whereas
A second possible explanation for the discrepancy is that re- (1998) suggest, we are challenged to create a cultural support
spondents, as a whole, were expressing more emotional loneli- system for the Fourth Age that allocates more resources to com-
ness rather than social loneliness (Weiss, 1973). The amount of pensate for the increasing dysfunctionality that occurs with age.
face-to-face contact with others (i.e., visiting) would be less rel- All professionals that care for or interact with older adults must
evant than having an intimate confidant, a relationship that be alert to the level of functioning and health in the individual.
could be maintained over the telephone. This explanation is Model replication relies on further analyses specifically exam-
consistent with the strong positive relationship between anxiety ining the moderating effects of cohort and of residential status,
and loneliness given that anxiety was in part assessed by emo- that is, dependent care versus independent living, and following
the type of loneliness experienced was dominantly social or Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1998). Savoir vivre in old age. National Forum,
emotional (Marangoni & Ickes, 1989). Second, with regard to 78, 13-18.
model evaluation, our chi-square statistic was significant in the Baron R. M, & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinc-
tion in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
nonmediated model. Examined alone, this would indicate a considerations. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.
poor fit of the model to the data (Bollen, 1989). However, given Bazargan, M., & Barbre, A. R. (1992). Self-reported memory problems among
the AGFI and GFI indices of fit (above .90) and a critical N of the Black elderly. Educational Gerontology, 18, 71-82.
200 (Hoelter, 1983), we believe our models are not only theo- Berg, S., Mellstrom, D., Persson, G., & Svanborg, A. (1981). Loneliness in the
Swedish aged. Journal of Gerontology, 36, 342-349.
retically but also statistically defensible. Bollen, K. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley
The statement that "correlation does not prove causation" & Sons.
(Bollen, 1989, p. 52) holds true for this model. The data in this Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure ofpsychological well-being. Chicago:
analysis represent one time period within the study, although Aldine.
future analyses with the data will take a longitudinal perspec- Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., & Tatsuoka, M. M. (1970). Handbook for Sixteen
Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF). Champaign, IL: Institute for
tive. As Bollen suggests, models can only approximate reality, Personality and Ability Testing.
which is what we have attempted to do. We are attempting to Christian, E., Dluhy, N., & O'Neill, R. (1989). Sounds of silence: Coping with
build a foundation for further model building which more accu- hearing loss and loneliness. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, 15, 4-9.
rately reflects the experiences of older Americans living inde- Creecy, R. E, Berg, W. E., & Wright, R. (1985). Loneliness among the elderly:
A causal approach. Journal of Gerontology, 40, 487-493.
pendently. By the same token, the "lack of a correlation does de Jong-Gierveld, J. (1987). Developing and testing a model of loneliness.
not disprove causation" (Bollen, 1989, p. 52). Consequently, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 119-128.
future data collection and analyses that are able to isolate, if Dugan, E., & Kivett, V. R. (1994). The importance of emotional and social iso-
possible, variables from other agents may indeed find our initial lation to loneliness among very old rural adults. The Gerontologist, 34,
model to be a valid representation of reality. Obviously replica- 340-346.
Falk, R. F., & Miller, N. B. (1992). A primer for soft modeling. Akron, OH:
tion is necessary. University of Akron Press.
The generalizability of this study is limited by the character- Fillenbaum, G. G. (1988). Multidimensional functional assessment of older
istics of the sample. As noted, all participants were living inde- adults: The Duke Older Americans Resources and Services procedures.
pendently and lived in the southern region of the United States; Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.
Fischer, C. S., & Phillips, S. L. (1982). Social characteristics of people with
consequently, an analysis of institutionalized older adults or small networks. In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A source-
adults from different regions of the country may produce re- book of current theory, research and therapy (pp. 21-39). New York: Wiley
sults different from those presented here. & Sons.
Despite these shortcomings, the implications of this study Folstein, M. F, Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: A
practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.
are clear. Loneliness does affect one's perceptions about health. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198.
As recognized by Christian, Dluhy, and O'Neill (1989), profes- Hoelter, J. W. (1983). The analysis of covariance structures: Goodness-of-fit in-
sionals who work with older adults must be attentive to this re- dices. Sociological Methods and Research, 11, 325-344.
lationship in their clients and keep in mind that alleviating lone- Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. (1994). Windows LISREL 8.14. Chicago, IL:
liness may be one way of affecting overall health. Among Scientific Software International Incorporated.
Kaufman, A. S. (1985). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Test-Revised. In J. V.
elderly persons living independently, loneliness is more preva- Mitchell, Jr. (Ed.), The ninth mental measurements yearbook (9th ed., pp.
lent with increasing age. Anxiety is particularly detrimental by 1694-1703). Lincoln, NE: The University of Nebraska Press.
increasing perceived loneliness and thus perceived ill-health. It Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Garner, W., Speicher, C. E., Penn, G., & Glaser, R. (1984).
is clear too that the telephone is a fundamental tool for older Psychosocial modifiers of immunocompetence in medical students.
adults living independently to maintain relationships. Increasing Psychosomatic Medicine, 46, 7-14.
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1992). Psychoneuroimmunology: Can psy-
sensory impairment, particularly hearing loss, therefore, is of chological interventions modulate immunity? Journal of Consulting and
concern to effective telephone usage. As Baltes and Baltes Clinical Psychology, 60, 569-575.
A MODEL OF LONELINESS P239
Lawton, M. T. (1975). The Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale: A revi- Reisberg, B., Ferris, S. H., De Leon, M. J., & Crook, T. (1982). The global de-
sion. Journal of Gerontology, 30, 85—89. terioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia.
Lopata, M. (1995). Loneliness. In G. L. Maddox, R. C. Atchley, J. G. Evans, C. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1136-1139.
E. Finch, D. F. Hultsch, R. A. Kane, M. D. Mezey, & E. C. Siegler (Eds.), Russell, D. (1996). UCLA loneliness scale (Version 3): Reliability, validity, and
The encyclopedia of aging (pp. 571-572). New York: Springer Publishing factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 20-40.
Company. Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Cutrona, C. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness
Marangoni, C, & Ickes, W. (1989). Loneliness: A theoretical review with im- scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality
plications for measurement. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480.
6, 93-128. Russell, D., Peplau, L. A., & Ferguson, M. L. (1978). Developing a measure of
Martin, P., Hagberg, B., & Poon, L. W. (1997). Predictors of loneliness in cen- loneliness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 42, 290-294.