Sample Size For Qualitative Research
Sample Size For Qualitative Research
Sample Size For Qualitative Research
32
Article ID:
20001202
Published:
December 2000
Author:
Peter DePaulo
Article Abstract
How large should the sample size be in a qualitative study? This article
discusses the importance of sample size in qualitative research.
In a qualitative research project, how large should the sample be? How
many focus group respondents, individual depth interviews (IDIs), or
ethnographic observations are needed?
We do have some informal rules of thumb. For example, Maria Krieger (in
her white paper, The Single Group Caveat, Brain Tree Research &
Consulting, 1991) advises that separate focus groups are needed for major
segments such as men, women, and age groups, and that two or more
groups are needed per segment because any one group may be
idiosyncratic. Another guideline is to continue doing groups or IDIs until we
seem to have reached a saturation point and are no longer hearing anything
new.
Such rules are intuitive and reasonable, but they are not solidly grounded
and do not really tell us what an optimal qualitative sample size may be. The
approach proposed here gives specific answers based on a firm foundation.
First, the importance of sample size in qualitative research must be
understood.
include a patient aggravated by the moody staffer turns out to be just over
one in three (0.349 to be exact). This probability is simple to calculate:
Consider that the chance of any one customer selected at random not being
a member of the 10 percent (aggravated) subgroup is 0.9 (i.e., a nine in 10
chance). Next, consider that the chance of failing to reach anyone from the
10 percent subgroup twice in a row (by selecting two customers at random)
is 0.9 X 0.9, or 0.9 to the second power, which equals 0.81. Now, it should
be clear that the chance of missing the subgroup 10 times in a row (i.e.,
when drawing a sample of 10) is 0.9 to the tenth power, which is 0.35.
Thus, there is a 35 percent chance that our sample of 10 would have
missed patients who experienced the staffer in a bad mood. Put another
way, just over one in three random samples of 10 will miss an experience or
characteristic with an incidence of 10 percent.
This seems counter-intuitively high, even to quant researchers to whom I
have shown this analysis. Perhaps people implicitly assume the fallacy that if
something has an overall frequency of one in N, then it is almost sure to
appear in N chances.
then a sample of nearly 50 would be needed. This would reduce the risk to
nearly 0.005 (see table).