Retrieve Study Peabody
Retrieve Study Peabody
Children’s vocabulary development is fundamental skill to assess. The strong relation between vocabulary
to their future language abilities. For example, children and other language skills suggests that, in practice, vocab-
with larger vocabularies have better phonological aware- ulary can be a convenient index of a child’s developmental
ness (Rvachew, 2006) and better morphological sensitivity level. Given the importance of vocabulary for future lin-
(Duncan, 2018; Mahony et al., 2000). When learning to guistic performance, clinicians and researchers need valid
read, vocabulary is a leading indicator on reading compre- and reliable means of measuring vocabulary knowledge.
hension (Brimo et al., 2018; Quinn et al., 2015). In addi- However, standardized assessments of receptive vocabu-
tion to the fact that vocabulary is related to a host of lary are often developed and normed for typically
other language abilities, vocabulary is a particularly easy developing, typically hearing children rather than children
with communication disorders such as hearing loss (HL),
Correspondence Erin M. Ingvalson: [email protected]. Disclosure:
who may be at elevated likelihood for language delays, or
The authors have declared that no competing financial or nonfinancial have atypical developmental trajectories in their language
interests existed at the time of publication. development. Although such assessments are frequently
1610 American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology • Vol. 32 • 1610–1619 • July 2023 • Copyright © 2023 The Authors
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.
used to assess vocabulary in children with HL, it is expressive vocabulary and precedes expressive vocabulary
unknown whether different tests yield similar characteriza- in development (e.g., Fenson et al., 1994). Additionally,
tions of delay and growth trajectory in this population. tests of receptive vocabulary do not have the same kinds
Here, we compare the vocabulary trajectories of pre- of performance demands as tests of expressive vocabulary,
schoolers with and without HL as measured by two com- which require children to pronounce a given word
monly administered receptive vocabulary assessments—the sufficiently correctly as to be recognized by the test
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Fifth Edition (PPVT-5; administrator—a skill that can be extra challenging for
Dunn, 2019) and the Receptive One-Word Picture young children with communicative disorders, including
Vocabulary Test–Fourth Edition (ROWPVT-4; Martin & HL. We similarly focus on receptive vocabulary here.
Brownell, 2011). Two common standardized tests that have been reported
in the literature are the PPVT-5 (henceforth, PPVT)
The Importance of Vocabulary in and the ROWPVT-4 (henceforth, ROWPVT). Both tests
Development use a four-alternative forced-choice design wherein the
child chooses the picture that best represents the stimulus
There is strong support for the idea that children’s word spoken by the examiner. Both tests also establish
vocabulary abilities are related to both their oral and writ- basal and ceiling metrics and utilize those metrics to
ten language skills. In particular, there is a bidirectional establish the raw score on the test. However, the rules for
association between vocabulary and phonological develop- establishing basal and ceiling metrics differ between the
ment. For example, while children more readily learn new two tests: The PPVT requires three consecutive correct
words made up of sounds that they can already produce responses for the basal versus eight consecutive correct
(e.g., Leonard, 1989), children’s vocabulary knowledge responses for the ROWPVT; the PPVT requires six con-
promotes children’s perception of phonological categories secutive incorrect responses for the ceiling versus six out
(e.g., Swingley, 2019). These associations between lexical of eight incorrect responses for the ROWPVT. The PPVT
and phonological processes can have cascading influences also has more test items than the ROWPVT (240 and
on long-term literacy outcomes as well. Having a large 190, respectively). Our experience administering the two
vocabulary gives children more information about the tests indicates that the PPVT takes longer to administer,
sounds that make up words, which can help them process although publisher estimates of administration times for
and subsequently learn new words that they encounter in both tests are 10–15 min.
spoken language (Morgan & Demuth, 1996; Pierrehumbert,
2003) and decode and learn new words in written lan- Both the PPVT and ROWPVT have been normed
guage (Duff et al., 2015; Lee, 2011). on and extensively used with typically developing children.
Both tests have also been used with a variety of children
This bidirectional association between vocabulary and identified as having a communication disorder or identi-
phonological sensitivity therefore is one factor that contrib- fied as being at an elevated likelihood of developing a
utes to the so-called rich-get-richer or “Matthew effect” communication disorder. Reliable and valid measurement
(Duff et al., 2015; Merton & Merton, 1968), such that chil- of vocabulary in children with an elevated risk of a com-
dren who know more words are more successful at learning munication disorder is particularly important, because
new words. Thus, vocabulary is associated with long-term these children often have smaller vocabularies (McGregor
literacy outcomes, such as reading comprehension, both et al., 2013, 2021) and greater weaknesses in word learn-
because it supports the phonological sensitivity skills needed ing than their typically developing peers (Gray, 2004;
to decode words, and because knowing more words allows Riches et al., 2005). Consistent with these findings, chil-
a child to recognize the meanings of familiar words and dren with early onset HL often show lower overall vocab-
infer the meanings of unfamiliar words they decode from ulary levels and shallower slopes of vocabulary growth
context. Furthermore, as many children with HL who use than children with normal hearing (NH; Ganek et al.,
cochlear implants or hearing aids and are learning a spoken 2012; Lund, 2016, 2019; Nott et al., 2009). Word learning
language have delays or difficulties in vocabulary develop- studies have demonstrated that children with HL require
ment, phonological sensitivity, and reading, it is important more presentations to learn a novel word than do their
to accurately measure vocabulary in this population. peers with NH (Houston et al., 2012; Walker & McGregor,
2013). Perhaps not surprisingly, then, children with HL
Assessing Receptive Vocabulary in Children also show weaker reading comprehension than children
With and Without HL with NH (Mayberry et al., 2011; Wendt et al., 2015;
Worsfold et al., 2018), a performance deficit that can
In much of the literature, vocabulary is often quanti- likely be at least partially attributed to children with HL’s
fied as receptive vocabulary, which is larger than a child’s lower vocabulary levels.
Characteristic HL NH
n (n males) 31 (10) 31 (15)
M age at enrollment in months (SD) 43.06 (3.89) 43.26 (3.58)
Hispanic 17% 20%
Asian 8% 5%
Black or African American 0% 0%
White 71% 90%
More than one race 21% 5%
Mothers with bachelor’s degree or higher 68% 89%
Fathers with bachelor’s degree or higher 74% 100%
M age at identification in months (SD) 2.93 (4.96) —
M age at 1st amplification in months (SD) 9.78 (10.28) —
n CI users 11 —
n HA users 17 —
n bone conduction users 3 —
n bimodal CI—HA users 0 —
Note. Dashes indicate not applicable. HL = hearing loss; NH = normal hearing; CI = cochlear implant; HA = hearing aid.
Figure 1. Standard scores on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) and Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWVPT) by
children with hearing loss or with normal hearing at T1 and T2.
hearing aids and children who used cochlear implants. We interpret these findings to support the use of
However, there was a significant effect of time for both either the ROWPVT or the PPVT in clinical practice,
standard scores and age equivalencies, potentially indicat- although caution should be used if testing vocabulary fre-
ing a susceptibility to practice effects. quently. The PPVT includes two test forms, which are
Figure 3. Standard scores on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) and Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWVPT) for
children who use hearing aids and children who use cochlear implants at T1 and T2.