Bauer 1957
Bauer 1957
Bauer 1957
Raymond A. Bauer
42
lands. Other techniques are indistinguishable from the shrewd common
sense practices of journalists and salesmen of all times and places, and
are based on psychological, physiological, and physical principles known
to interrogators regardless of national or political allegiance, or of any
academic persuasion. On this last point I have the welcome, although
unsolicited, support of Comrade Khrushchev, who summarized the
S t a l i t formula for obtaining confessions as “beat, beat, beat.” I shall
refrain from attempting to interpret this within the Pavlovian schema.
My direct evidence for Bolshevik interest in or use of psychological
or psychiatric techniques in political matters pertains to the pre-Stalinist
period. “Brainwashing” dates from the very period for which the weight
of evidence is to the contrary.
The Stalinist approach to manipulation of people was such as to
make it unlikely that the Bolsheviks of that period would turn to academic
psychologists or psychiatrists for principles of propaganda, interrogation,
etc. For this combination of reasons I a m inclined to look for more obvious
(less devious) explanations of the effectiveness of brainwashing. It is not
unprecedented in human affairs that laymen have developed effective
means of coercion and persuasion. Work by a number of scholars has
shown that the effectiveness of Communist techniques of persuasion and
coercion is quite understandable in the light of our own notions of human
behavior, regardless of whether or not they were based on a n explicit
psychological theory.* Furthermore, as Schein has commented in connec-
tion with the Chinese treatment of American POWs, the distinguishing
thing about these techniques is their number and variety, not their appar-
ent derivation from a single theoretical point of view.
At this point I should make clear that when I refer to “brainwash-
ing” I am talking about persuasion as opposed to coercion. No one
appears to be puzzled by the fact that punishment or threats of punish-
ment can induce men to colllaborate or to produce confessions of things
they did not do. Such men are not admired, but they are perceived as
acting from understandable though despised motives. The mysterious
feature of “brainwashing” is that people’s beliefs can be changed. Coercion
can affect behavior, but what can persuade a man to change his beliefs?
I have specifically called attention to the distinction between coercion
and persuasion, and have also treated behavior and belief in the same
way, because we habitually tend to take for granted that these concepts
are in opposition to each other. By doing this,I have intended to set the
stage for introducing the observation that in Communist techniques of
indoctrination, coercion and persuasion, behavior and belief, tend to be-
come indistinguishable. In a recent brilliant article ’ Paul Kecskemeti
makes the very insightful point that the Communists seldom attempt se-
rious indoctrination of a person until they have control over his behavior
( 7 ) . Stalin referred to coercion as “the prerequisite of persuasion” (8).
44
the Soviet state under Stalin exercised over the life situation of each in-
&dual. I would not for a moment imply that there were not many
persons who would have been spontaneously and “naturally1’ loyal in the
absence of coercion-although after Khrushchev’s recent utterances one
m y indeed be puzzled as to who they were. Nor, would I attempt for a
moment to deny the importance of other psychological processes. But in
my way of W i n g , the most important thing to understand is how the
potentially disloyal person responded to a situation in which he had no
viable alternative except to accommodate-how he not only controlled his
behavior, but began to control his thoughts and attitudes.
To some extent one has to arrive at this inferentially, but a n impres-
sive proportion of the refugees whom we interviewed asserted: “I pushed
my doubts into the background.” Oleg, whose caSe was recently reported
by Helen Beier and me in the lournal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
(3), described eloquently how he found himself trying to convince others
of the rectitude of the Soviet order, only to realize afterward that these
attempts to convince others were really attempts to still his own doubts.
Realizing that one’s livelihood, and in many instances one’s life, depends
on his political propriety, the individual’s own thoughts become dangerous
property. One man complained of life aboard ship because of “the danger
that you might say something against the regime while asleep.” Another
cited as a reason for not returning to the Soviet Union after the war the
fact that because he had seen the countries of Western Europe “it would
be harder for me to keep quiet.” Another says of the doubts he had while
he was still an oficer in the Soviet Army:
“If you kept these things in your head they might come out some day.
You were afraid that you might tell somebody. Therefore, very few people
in the Soviet Union think about these things.”
I said that some of the mechanisms of self-conversion had to be in-
ferred. Some of the strongest inferential evidence came from case histories
of persons who survived many traumatic events while living in the Soviet
system, but reported no predominantly disloyal thoughts or behavior for
the period of their residence under Soviet power. In many instances they
sought out positions of responsibility and privilege and reported their
pride in being accepted as “full-fledged citizens.” These same persons,
however, reported sudden disillusionment and/or antagonism toward the
Soviet order almost immediately after being removed from Soviet jurisdic-
tion-say, by being captured by the German army. One could only con-
clude from these stories that these anti-Soviet feelings which welled up all
of a sudden were feelings which had always been there, but had system-
atically been repressed for as long as they jeopardized the individual’s
chances for survival and accommodation.
Not only does the individual have to control his disloyal thoughts,
but most Soviet citizens are in a position wherein they must affirm their
loyalty positively by appropriate statements and behavior. Again, I am not
contending that there are no loyal Soviet citizens who accept the officiaI
45
ideology easily and naturally. However, I was struck by the corrosive effect
of forced conformity on those whose loyalty had at one time or another
been shaken. The life stories of Soviet refugees reflect what we would
expect from some of the experiments on compliant behavior in small
groups and the work of Janis and King. Where there is a positive motive
for adopting the point of view which one is forced to espouse overtly, he
comes to “accept it” privately.
Of course, all of these effects become exaggerated in persons under
interrogation in prisons. The immediate threat of losing one’s life or a t
least one’s liberty, or of jeopardizing other persons, heightens the desire
to conform if one can do so with a modicum of decency. To this must
be added the effect of excessive fatigue, the confusion engendered by hav-
ing been examined and cross-examined in a detailed fashion on the same
topics many times, and the ability of the interrogators to feed in false and
confusing information. To any one who is curious about reactions to such
conditions, I would suggest reading Russian Purge, by Beck and Godin
( 2 ) . Contrary to the stereotype of books by refugees, this is an extraordi-
narily objective, insightful, and detailed account of a number of inter-
rogations, together with interesting speculations as to the psychological
processes at work among the prisoners. After a while many prisoners be-
came thoroughly confused as to just what they had said and done under
certain circumstances in the past and as to their own motives. Some be-
came at least partially persuaded of their guilt. But, persuaded or not,
they eventually were convinced that there was nothing left for them to do
but to write a confession.
Clearly the life of the ordinary Soviet citizen, and the conditions of
interrogation by the Soviet secret police, are by no means exact equiva-
lents of the situation of American POWs in the hands of the Chinese
Communists, or of the “reeducational institutions” which Dr. Lifton
studied. However, in varying degrees we find essentially the same tech-
niques employed by Communist officials in all these situations, and-again
with variations-the same sorts of reactions on the part of their victims.
Every instance of human behavior is properly the subject of psychological
study. And if any particular batch of behavior attracts our attention in a
special way, I suppose that is all the more reason to study it. The work
of my colleagues on the panel has done much to make psychological sense
out of brainwashing, and has made it clear that it can be understood as
psychology, not demonology. When one considers the trouble to which the
Communists have gone-in these various situations-to produce collab-
oration, confession, or change of ideology, I see little to wonder about the
fact that they are-again in varying degrees-successful. For that matter,
there is more to remark in their lack of success in persuading American
POWs, than in the sinister influence of “brainwashing.”
I find myself personally interested in why the Communists were will-
ing to devote so much energy to the conversion of persons who disagreed
with them, and in why portions of the American public (inasmuch as we
46
can judge from press reports) were so upset at the Communists’ limited
Success in converting a few American POWs. It is possible that we share
in common with the Communists a lack of security in our own ideological
beliefs, even though we both proclaim our confidence that we have the
true way of life. A single American soldier converted to Communism could
arouse feelings of anxiety and guilt over one’s own repressed ideological
doubts. Hence, our eagerness to attribute such conversions to the demonic
machinations of the Doctors Pavlov and Fu Manchu. The Communists,
in the role of proselytizers, assert that any sensible man, once freed of the
crippling influence of bourgeois-capitalist ideology, will accept their view
of things. By devoting vast amounts of energy to converting every heretic,
they attempt to make this a self-fulfilling prophecy. We can continue to
expect this behavior on their part.
I hope that we, however, may change toward a more mature, con-
fident acceptance of diversity of political views.
REFERENCES
(1) Bauer, Raymond A. T h e New Man in Soviet Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.:
Harvard University Press, 1952.
(2) Beck, F. (pseudonym) and Godin, W. (pseudonym). Russian Purge and the
Extraction of Confessions. New York: Viking Press, 1951.
(3) Beier, Helen, and Bauer, Raymond A. Oleg: A Member of the Soviet ‘‘Gold-
en Youth.’’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1955, 51, 139-145.
( 4 ) Bettelheim, Bruno. Individual and Mass Behavior in Extreme Situations. In
Swanson, Guy E., Newcomb, Theodore M., and Hartley, Eugene L. (eds.).
Readings in Social Psychology. 2nd edition. New York: Henry Holt, 1952.
( 5 ) Jahoda, Marie, and Cook, Stuart. Security Measures and Freedom of Thought.
Yale Law Journal, 1953, 61, 295-333.
( 6 ) Janis, Irving, and King, Bert T., et. al. The Influence of Role Playing on
Opinion Change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1954, 49,
211-218.
( 7 ) Kecskemeti, Paul. The Soviet Approach to International Political Commu-
nication. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1956, 20.
(8) Studenkin, Semen S., et. al. Soviet Administrative Law. Moscow: 1950, 170.
47