Previewpdf
Previewpdf
Previewpdf
Kenneth W. Merrell
The University of Iowa
Gretchen A. Gimpel
Utah State University
~~I~~~~~i?G~XP Press
NEW YORK AND LONDON
First published 1998 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Published 2014 by Psychology Press
711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017
Psychology Press is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business
Copyright © 1998 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Publisher's Note
The publisher has gone to great lengths to ensure the quality of this reprint
but points out that some imperfections in the original may be apparent.
Contents
Preface . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vii
References 182
Author Index 197
Subject Index . 203
v
This page intentionally left blank
Preface
The scientific study and clinical applications of child and adolescent social
skills became important and well-established endeavors in the behavioral sci-
ences as well as in the educational and human services arenas during the
1980s and 1990s. The increasing prominence of these applications of child
and adolescent social skills is partly because the field has finally matured to
the point that there is now an extensive literature base, making it possible to
identify the most important scientific and applied components. Another rea-
son for the surge of interest and popularity in this area has resulted from the
increasing recognition of the critical importance of social-emotional devel-
opment to individuals and society in general. In his 1994 best-selling popular
book, Emotional Intelligence, Daniel Goleman persuasively argued that the
social-emotional aspects of human functioning have a tremendous impact on
individual lives and society and perhaps may be more important to our happi-
ness and success in the long run than intelligence or IQ as it is traditionally
defined.
Evidence of this critical impact can be found virtually anywhere in the ru-
ined relationships, academic and occupational failure, and violence and de-
spair that so often characterize the lives of individuals who have serious
deficits in social competence or social skills. Knowing that the roots of these
social-emotional problems are usually planted firmly in childhood is both
troubling and promising: troubling because it underscores the failure of our
institutions and families to ensure that all children will realize our hopes for
vii
viii PREFACE
Brandon, a 7-year-old boy in the first grade, was referred to the school child
study team. Brandon's teacher reported that he had extreme difficulty getting
along with peers and that he was often physically aggressive. The first author
of this book, a member of the team, was asked to observe Brandon in the class-
room and on the playground. The classroom observation took place during an
academic activity in which the students were writing spelling words from the
chalkboard onto their papers. Brandon quickly lost interest in the activity and
tried to get the attention of a student sitting behind him. Cupping his hands to
his mouth and making a strange face, Brandon repeatedly whispered "hey
you!" to his neighbor. When this ploy failed to get the attention of the other
student, Brandon poked him in the shoulder with a sharpened pencil, which
resulted in a loud cry of "stop it!" and a warning from the teacher. The play-
ground observation took place during morning recess on a very frigid January
day. Most of the 60 or so students simply stood near the building, huddled with
their hands in their pockets. Brandon and about 10 others elected to play on
the jungle gym and tunnel slide. During the 10-minute observation period, it
was noted that Brandon engaged in 24 different acts of physical aggression
(hitting, shoving, kicking, and the like) while continually making verbal
threats or insults to his peers. Within a few weeks, a full-blown special educa-
tion eligibility assessment was in process. During an interview and testing ses-
1
2 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
sion when Brandon was asked how he got along with other kids, he became
stoic, appeared to hold back tears, and tersely stated, "They all hate me."
Sarah, a lO-year-old girl in the fourth grade, had been a primary concern of
her homeroom teacher for most of the school year. A classic wallflower, Sarah
was always on the sidelines of social activity and was described by her teach-
ers during lunchroom discussions as being painfully shy. She seldom initiated
any social interactions, and although she was never in trouble for inappropri-
ate or antisocial behavior, she appeared to have no close friends. During the
spring parent-teacher conferences, Sarah's teacher discussed her concerns
with Sarah's mother, who looked as reserved and quiet as Sarah. Stating that
this was Sarah's typical mode of behavior, her mother noted that Sarah had
few friends, and usually became involved in solitary play activities at home,
even though she seemed to want friends desperately. A few sessions with the
school counselor were recommended and, with some initial hesitation, Sarah
cooperated. During the third session, Sarah stated that the prospect of ap-
proaching children or initiating a conversation often resulted in such thoughts
as "I don't know how to do this and I'm going to look like an idiot!"
Casey, a 16-year-old boy in the tenth grade, was referred to a clinical psy-
chologist for treatment of depression after admission to a hospital emergency
room for an unsuccessful suicide attempt. (He became unconscious after he
drank several glasses of wine and took a number of his mother's tranquiliz-
ers.) During the course of treatment, some facts about Casey emerged. He
had been depressed for nearly 4 months, and the depression appeared to be
precipitated by two events: the death of his grandfather and failure to make
the junior varsity basketball team. The course of treatment focused on two pri-
mary areas: getting Casey involved in positive or enjoyable activities again and
helping him to change the way he was thinking about things. Casey reported
that he seldom did things with friends anymore and that when he was around
people, he often avoided conversation and usually failed to make eye contact
in his reply when spoken to.
These three cases appear to have little in common, but they share a collec-
tive thread: Brandon, Sarah, and Casey all exhibited deficits in basic social
skills, and as a result they all are suffering. These cases also serve as a starting
point for this introductory chapter aimed at providing a basic definition and
understanding of social skills, showing why they are important, and explain-
ing how their presence, absence, or adequacy during childhood and adoles-
cence may have powerful and far-reaching implications, not only during the
formative years but throughout the course of the life span.
Argyle, 1981 Social behavior that is effective in realizing the goals of the inter-
actors
Combs & Slaby, 1977 The ability to interact with others in a given social context in
specific ways that are socially acceptable or valucd and at the
same time personally or mutually beneficial
Foster & Ritchey, 1979 Those responses that within a given situation maximize the proba-
bility of maintaining or enhancing positive effects for the inter-
actor
Gresham, 1981 Behaviors that maximize the probability of reinforcement and de-
crease the likelihood of punishment contingent on one's social
behavior
Gresham & Elliott, 1987 Behaviors that in given situations predict important social out-
comes
Hargie, Saunders, The skills employed when interacting at an interpersonal level
& Dickson, 1987 with other people, which are goal-directed, interrelated, appropri-
ate to the situation. identifiable units of behavior, and learned
behaviors
Kelly, 1982 Identifiable learned behaviors that individuals use in interpersonal
situations to obtain maximum reinforcement from their environ-
ment
Libet & Lewinsohn, 1973 The complex ability to emit behaviors that are positively or nega-
tively reinforced and not to emit behaviors that are punished or
extinguished by others
Matson, 1988 A socially skilled person, who is much more likely to receive the
types of reinforcers generally considered to be socially acceptable
or desirable
Mcguire & Priestley, 1981 Those kinds of behavior that are basic to effective face-to-face
communication between individuals
Shepherd, 1983 An individual's observed behavior during a social interaction,
characterized by the ability to sustain social roles and relation-
ships
Schlundt & McFall, 1985 The specific component processes that enable an individual to
behave in a manner that will be judged as competent
Schneider, Rubin, The nexus between the individual and the environment; the tools
& Ledingham, 1985 used to initiate and sustain the peer relations that are a vital part
of psychological well-being
Trower, 1979 Individual targets or goals sought to obtain rewards, and goal
attainment dependent on skilled behavior
Young & West, 1984 Specific positive interpersonal behaviors that lead to desirable
social outcomes
4
DEFINING SOCIAL SKILLS 5
Regardless of how social skills are defined, a working definition of the con-
struct is only one aspect necessary for understanding. To get a better idea of
the tangible or practical aspects of social skills, it is important to look at the
concrete examples in this chapter, which provide tangible frameworks for
viewing social skills.
A good general framework of basic social skills categories and subcate-
gories was developed for a social skills training program produced by
Stephens (1978) and later detailed by Cartledge and Milburn (1986). By the
6 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
TABLE l.2
Four General Categories and 30 Subcategories of
Social Skills'"
use of a task analysis method, social skills were grouped into four major cate-
gories and further analyzed into 30 subcategories. These general categories
and subcategories are detailed in Table 1.2.
Obviously, even the 30 subcategories in this social skills breakdown could
be divided and broken down even further. Stephens (1978) actually identified
136 specific social skills that fit into these general categories and subcate-
gories. Using the major category of interpersonal behaviors and the subcat-
egory of coping with conflict as an example, we see that six specific social
skills can be identified:
1. Responds to teasing or name calling by ignoring, changing the subject, or
using some other constructive means
2. Responds to physical assault by leaving the situation, calling for help, or
using some other constructive means
3. Walks away from peer when angry to avoid hitting
4. Refuses the request of another politely
5. Expresses anger with nonaggressive words rather than physical action or
aggressive words
6. Constructively handles criticism or punishment perceived as undeserved.
EXAMPLES OF SOCIAL SKILLS 7
Of course, these six specific social skills do not exhaust the behaviors in-
volved in the skill of being able to cope with conflict in an appropriate man-
ner, but they serve to provide a good example of how social skills can be con-
ceptualized at different levels ranging from the macro- (wide) to the micro-
(narrow) spectrum.
The most likely places to find breakdowns of social skills categories and
their specific behaviors are in social skills training curriculums, which are typ-
ically developed through task analysis procedures. Later chapters on social
skills training in this volume provide more specific concrete examples of these
sorts of task-specific social skills. Another way of identifying specific compo-
nent behaviors or tasks within categories of social skills is through the use of
multivariate statistical techniques such as factor analysis, a procedure em-
ployed more often in developing social skills assessment instruments than in
the development of social skills training programs. More detail on the behav-
ioral dimensions approach to identification of social skills clusters or dimen-
sions is presented in later in this chapter.
HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENTS
their parents and not differing from adults in terms of thought processes and
mental development. In the Western world, it was not until the late 18th and
early 19th centuries that childhood was viewed as a unique developmental pe-
riod and that individuals with severe behavioral and emotional problems were
given treatment that we would consider humane or even marginally effective.
In detailing some of the historical antecedents in the area of children's so-
cial skills, Rubin and Ross (1982) noted that the 1930s was a period of exten-
sive activity in the area of developmental psychology, and that much of this ac-
tivity was focused on children's social behavior. Important historical figures
such as Mead, Piaget, Parten, and Moreno all made important contributions
to the study of children's social behavior, and some of this work resulted in
practical efforts to implement assessment and intervention systems for child
social behavior. For example, Moreno's influential (and ominously titled)
book Who Shall Survive? (1934) is generally credited as the first systematic
published work on the use of sociometric assessment, and the journal So-
ciometry was first published in the 1930s.
It appears that the 1940s and 1950s were a period of retrenchment or hi-
bernation in this area. The pressures of World War II no doubt contributed to
the decline of work in the area of children's social behavior during the 1940s,
and the political climate of the Cold War and Sputnik era of the 1950s proba-
bly added further to this decline. A strong academic emphasis on the biologic
and physical sciences were the norm during this period.
The 1960s saw a resurgence of interest in children's social behavior, led in
part by the rediscovery of Piaget's work and spurred on by such sociopolitical
developments as the "War on Poverty," that resulted in the implementation of
the Head Start program. However, the focus of interest during this period was
most prominently on children's cognitive behavior. According to Rubin and
Ross (1982), it was not until the early 1970s that children's social behavior
again took the forefront in education and psychology. The publication of the
third edition of Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology (Mussen, 1970)
and the appearance of highly influential research reports on the long-term
effects of children's social skills and peer relations (Cowen, Pederson, Babi-
gan, Izzo, & Trost, 1973; Roff, Sells, & Golden, 1972) were watershed events
in this area.
Since the 1980s, high-quality research on children's social behavior has
continued, but the most significant developments in this area may be the prac-
tical implementation or translation of this body of work into screening, as-
sessment, and intervention programs for use in the classroom and clinic. It is
unusual to attend large professional conferences in child/school psychology
or special education in which social skills assessment and training are not
prominently featured among conference topics. Any survey of professional
materials catalogs also reveals the prominent applied place that children's so-
cial behavior now occupies in the fields of education and psychology. We are
HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENTS 9
currently in a period of rapid growth in this field. Among the issues yet to be
resolved and that still need attention are the development of a sound classifi-
cation taxonomy for children's social skills, the development of ecologically
valid assessment tools directly linked to intervention, and an increase in our
ability to conduct social skills training in an effective and generalizable man-
ner. These current issues are all specifically addressed in subsequent chapters
of this volume.
Our discussion so far has provided some basic definitions of social skills, out-
lined some examples of specific social skills categories and tasks, and looked
at the historical antecedents of this field. We now move on to analyze the re-
lationship between social skills and a closely related construct: social compe-
tence. Although the terms social skills and social competence are often used
interchangeably, most experts in the field agree that they are independent, al-
beit related, constructs. The key differences between social skills and social
competence and how the two construct.'l are thought to be related has been ar-
ticulated in a frequently cited article by McFall (1982). According to McFall,
social skills are the specific behaviors that an individual must exhibit to per-
form competently on a given task. On the other hand, social competence is an
evaluative or summary term based on conclusions or judgments that the per-
son has performed the task adequately. These judgments are typically based
on the opinions of others (e.g., peers, parents, and teachers) or comparisons
to some explicit criteria or some normative group (Gresham, 1986). As Mc-
Fall explained, the presence of social competence does not necessarily imply
that there has been exceptional performance of social skills; it merely indi-
cates that social skills performance has been adequate. The evaluative or sum-
mary aspect of social competence has also been supported by Hops (1983),
who defined the construct as "a summary term which reflects social judgment
about the general quality of an individual's performance in a given situation"
(p.3).
Hops and Finch (1985) also wrote on the distinction between social skill
and social competence, producing an analysis that generally concurs with Mc-
Fall's (1982) conceptualization. They also pointed out that social competence
consists of both nonsocial and social skills. For example, motor skills behav-
iors (nonsocial skills) are generally not thought of as being within the domain
of social competence, yet they tend to be positively related to children's social
competence as measured by peer sociometric assessments. One study cited
to support this notion is that conducted by Broekhoff (1977), who demon-
strated that ball-throwing distance was the best predictor of social status in el-
ementary-age boys, and that this relationship remained quite stable over time.
10 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
each of the factors that comprised the dimension were listed (e.g., all of the
items comprising the first Peer Relations factor). Second, individual items of
the next Peer Relations factor were listed, with similar items grouped together.
This process was carried out for all five of the most common dimensions.
Third, similar social skills were grouped together to determine the principal
behavioral characteristics (those occurring in one third or more of the stud-
ies) associated with each dimension. Finally, these principal social skills were
rank ordered (based on frequency) as they appear in Table 1.3.
The peer relations dimension occurred in 11 (52.38%) of the studies. This
dimension appears to be dominated by social skills that reflect a child or
youth who is positive with his or her peers. Such skills as complimenting or
praising others, offering help or assistance, and inviting others to play or in-
teract appear to describe this dimension well.
The self-management dimension also occurred in 11 studies. This dimen-
sion reflects a child or youth who might be labeled by others as emotionally
well adjusted and able to control his or her temper, follow rules and limits, com-
promise with others, and receive criticism well.
The academic skills dimension occurred in 10 ( 47.62%) of the studies. This
dimension is dominated by social skills seen in a child or youth who might be
called an independent and productive worker by a teacher. Such skills as ac-
complishing tasks or assignments independently, completing individual as-
TABLE l.3
The Five Most Common Dimensions of Social Skills'"
,', Developed from a review and analysis by Caldarella and Merrell (1997).
CLASSIFICATION OF SOCIAL SKILLS 13
signments, and carrying out teacher directions all appear to describe this di-
mension well.
The compliance dimension occurred in 8 (38.09%) of the studies. Pictured
here is a child who essentially gets along with others by following rules and ex-
pectations, appropriately using free time, and sharing things. Essentially, this
dimension involves complying with appropriate requests made by others.
The assertion dimension occurred in 7 (33.33%) of the studies reviewed.
This dimension is dominated by social skills evidenced by a child or youth who
might be called outgoing or extroverted by others. Such skills as initiating con-
versations with others, acknowledging compliments, and inviting others to in-
teract all appear to describe this dimension well.
These five most common dimensions of child and adolescent social skills
have a strong base of empirical support, being derived in more than one third
of the studies reviewed, with two derived in more than half of the studies. No
other known research has done such an extensive review of empirically de-
rived social skill dimensions of children and adolescents. Indeed, it could be
said that this review is breaking new ground by applying the influential research
method used by Quay (1986), which combines aspects of both meta-analysis
and qualitative review to an area of critical importance: child and adolescent
positive social behaviors.
Because these dimensions of social skills have been identified so frequently
over the past 20 years of research, practitioners and researchers should con-
sider focusing on these areas for assessment and intervention. Many of the so-
cial skills subsumed by these dimensions have already been incorporated into
excellent, well-validated assessment (Merrell, 1994a; Walker, Colvin, & Ram-
sey, 1995) and intervention (McGinnis & Goldstein, 1984) strategies. What
this study provides is further empirical support for the five essential social
skills dimensions comprising the taxonomy. Gesten (1976) noted that com-
petencies in clients must be identified and reinforced to maximize treatment
and research outcomes. Perhaps this review and the resulting taxonomy can
help to identify appropriate behaviors to reinforce and balance the scales be-
tween assessing for both positive and negative behaviors in children and ado-
lescents.
As defined in this chapter, theories or models of social skills and social com-
petence refer to how these concepts look or they way they are constructed.
Theories and models of how social skills and competence develop and are
demonstrated are discussed separately in chapter 2. For the purposes of this
discussion, the term model is used, which can be thought of as a paradigm of
....
"'"
Social Competence
Peer
Adaptive Behavior Social Skills
Acceptance
I I I
Independent Interpersonal Self-related Task-related
Physical Language Academic
Functioning Behaviors Behaviors Berlaviors
Development Development Competencies
Skills
I I I
FIG. l.l. A working model of social competence originally conceptualized by Reschly and
Gresham (1981) and expanded by Gresham (1986) and by Gresham and Reschly (1987a).
THEORIES AND MODELS 15
how social skills and social competence appear and what constitutes their es-
sential components. However, before exploring these ideas further, it is im-
portant to recognize that all the models we explore are hypothetical and thus
somewhat speculative in nature. As already stated, psychological constructs
are notoriously difficult demonstrate empirically. Thus, the ideas postulated
herein must be considered as good working models based on the best evidence
currently available, but not the definitive models hoped for in the future.
One of the most influential models of social competence in the school psy-
chology, special education, and child development literature is that proposed
by Reschly and Gresham (1981) and expanded further by Gresham (1986)
and by Gresham and Reschly (1987a). According to the original model, social
competence is considered to be the superordinate construct composed of two
subordinate constructs: adaptive behavior and social skills. As the visual rep-
resentation of this model indicates (Fig. 1.1), adaptive behavior for children is
thought to consist of independent functioning skills, physical development,
language development, and academic competencies. Social skills, however,
are thought to consist of three major subcomponents: interpersonal behav-
iors (e.g., acceptance of authority, conversation skills, cooperative behaviors,
and play behaviors), self-related behaviors (e.g., expression of feelings, ethi-
cal behavior, and a positive attitude toward self), and task-related behaviors
(e.g., attending behaviors, completing tasks, following directions, and work-
ing independently). To the original model conceptualized by Reschly and
Gresham (1981), Gresham (1986) added a third subordinate component of
social competence: peer acceptance. As proposed in this model, the peer ac-
ceptance component is considered to be a part, but also an outcome or result,
of socially competent behavior (Gresham & Elliott, 1987; Gresham & Reschly,
1987a). Thus, peer acceptance is seen as something separate from adaptive
behavior and social skills under the broad model of social competence, but
also as something that results from the adequate performance of these two
other components.
Although this notion of social competence has been quite influential and
appears to be based on a sound theoretical and empirical rationale, it is by no
means definitive or universally accepted. Some potential problems inherent
in the effort to identify the best model for a theory of social competence and
social skills can be explored by looking at literature defining adaptive behav-
ior, which already has been hypothesized as the second subordinate construct
in the social competence model. Early in the 20th century, Edgar Doll (1935)
discussed what we now refer to as the construct of adaptive behavior in widely
influential writings that included references to factors known today as social
competence and social skills. The construct of adaptive behavior eventually
became a keystone in the definition of mental retardation, with the most
widely accepted definitions considering deficits in adaptive behavior to be
among its central defining components (American Association on Mental Re-
tardation, 1992; Grossman, 1983).
16 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
was a central component of emotional adjustment and mental health and that
a child who developed the ability to interact successfully with others early in
life was likely to develop a personality style characterized by highly adaptive
and successful social behaviors. In summary, the amount of theoretical and
empirical work focusing on the positive outcomes of good social skills pales
beside the literature on deficits and problems, but it still lays an important
foundation of understanding.
A most intriguing and widely cited study in this area was conducted by
Cowen, Pederson, Babigan, Izzo, and Trost (1973). These researchers con-
ducted an 11- to 13-year follow-up of Grade 3 children identified as "vulnera-
ble" or at "clinical risk," based largely on peer problems, social isolation, and
social rejection from peers. At follow-up, these children were found to have
disproportionate rates of appearances in a community-wide psychiatric regis-
ter (e.g., they were diagnosed and received services for psychiatric disorders
at a substantially higher rate than their control peers who were not identified
as vulnerable or at risk due to the social behavior criteria). Interestingly, the
single best predictor of later psychiatric difficulty in this study was negative
peer judgment using sociometric assessment techniques from a method re-
ferred to as the "class play" (discussed in detail in chapter 3). The assignment
of negative roles in a hypothetical class play included such things as being
nominated to play the role of someone who is too bossy, not liked by other
people, in trouble, and so forth.
In summary, it has been well established that poor social adjustment and
acceptance during childhood is a reasonably strong predictor of psychiatric or
mental health problems later in life, and that individuals who exhibit psychi-
atric problems often are notably lacking or deficient in basic social skills.
Although this link has been empirically established for some time, under-
standing the true nature or direction of this relationship is a somewhat more
complex matter.
Social Skills and Externalizing Problems. Perhaps the most strongly es-
tablished link between social skills problems and poor outcomes has been
found in the area of externalizing disorders, which by definition include a
broad array of acting out and other-directed problems such as aggressive and
antisocial behavior, hyperactivity, and the range of specific problems that are
part of the conduct disorder syndrome (Quay, 1986). This syndrome and other
externalizing disorders are worth a special note because of their immensity
and expense. Matson and Ollendick (1988) cited a variety of evidence sug-
gesting that externalizing conduct problems are the most common reason for
behavior therapy referral, represent about one third to one half of all family
and school referrals for mental health services, and probably cost society
more than any other type of social-emotional problems.
Some of the most widely influential research on the link between social
skills problems in childhood and later externalizing conduct problems was
conducted by Roff (1961) and his colleagues during the 1960s and 1970s.
These researchers found that high rates of delinquency during adolescence
were strongly associated with social skills deficits and peer relationship prob-
lems, and that individuals discharged from military service because of bad
conduct were likely to show a similar pattern of deficits and problems (Roff,
Sells, & Golden, 1972). Ullman (1975) also demonstrated that teacher and
20 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
how to initiate and maintain friendships, but tend to exhibit behavioral re-
sponses perceived as unfriendly (Grenell, Glass, & Katz, 1987).
An interesting study in this area was conducted by Landau and Milich
(1988), who developed a semistructured TV talk show game in which each
child was given the opportunity to play the roles of both host and guest. In con-
trast to the non-ADHD children, those with ADHD were more likely to ask too
many questions when they were in the role of the guest, and to ask too few
questions when they were in the role of the host. In commenting on this study
and similar studies, Landau and Moore (1991) suggested that youngsters with
ADHD may be deficient in the skill of modulating their social communications
and thus, may have production or performance deficiencies rather than skill
deficits. In summary, youngsters with ADHD are also likely to have problems
in performing appropriate social behaviors, but these problems may be more
related to performance deficits than to social skill acquisition deficits.
What Causes What? This section focused on the negative outcomes asso-
ciated with inadequate social skills and placed a particular emphasis on psychi-
atric, externalizing, and internalizing disorders. With each of these problem
areas it was demonstrated that social skills deficits are often a component of
the syndrome. These findings lead to an unavoidable question: Do social skills
deficits cause these various behavioral and emotional problems or are social
skills deficits caused by these problems. Some theories (particularly the ear-
lier and one-dimensional theories) postulate a definitive answer using either
one of these two possible connections. For example, Lewinsohn's (1974) highly
influential behavioral model of depression (particularly in its earlier forms)
relies strongly on the notion that a disruption of pleasurable or positive events
(i.e., a lack of social reinforcement due to a lack of or decrement in social
skills) causes the syndrome of behaviors we refer to as depression.
The position most in favor currently, which the authors also advocate, is the
notion that social skills deficits both cause and are caused by various behav-
ioral and emotional problems. This notion is based on a model of mutual in-
fluence that is very consistent with Bandura's (1978) theory of reciprocal de-
terminism. To visualize how this model of causation actually works, recall the
case of Brandon, the 7 -year-old boy with social skills deficits and significant
antisocial and aggressive behaviors described at the beginning of this chapter.
Brandon's early development may have been characterized by inadequate
social skills acquisition coupled with his learning through coercive processes
that aggressive behavior sometimes gets him what he wants. In the school set-
ting, Brandon's inept attempts to approach peers and develop friendships re-
sults in some preliminary rejection from peers. Brandon responds to this pre-
liminary rejection through a mechanism that in this case is maladaptive: ag-
gressive and antisocial behavior directed toward his peers partially to get their
attention and partially to punish them for rejecting him. These antisocial and
aggressive behaviors lead to further alienation from the peer group, which in
turn leads to an escalating pattern of hostility and aggression directed out-
ward toward other children. The resulting isolation from peers, who might oth-
erwise serve as positive social role models for Brandon, further inhibits his ac-
quisition of social skills because he seldom observes positive social behavior
directed toward him by his peers, who have learned to fear and avoid him.
24 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
Thus, the escalating and insidious cycle continues to perpetuate itself. Bran-
don's deficits in social skills, in a sense, have caused an increase in his exter-
nalizing problem behaviors, which also serve as a causal factor in Brandon's
continuing pattern of social isolation and poor skills acquisition.
This discussion would not be complete without a brief excursion into an area
of current thought regarding social skills that has captured the imagination of
the public in recent years, namely, the notion that social skills may reflect a
specific type of practical human intelligence. For many years theoreticians,
researchers, and clinicians working in the area of human intelligence have
given at least peripheral attention to the idea that social-behavioral function-
ing may comprise a distinct form of what has been referred to as practical in-
telligence (Sternberg, 1986). For example, consider the administration of the
Comprehension subtest on the various versions of the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for Children. A hallmark task on this subtest involves asking the child in
effect: "What is the thing to do if someone who is younger than you wants to
fight with you?" To receive full credit for answering this question, the child
must in some way display self-restraint in his or her answer, recognizing the
unfairness of a fight with a younger child. Obviously, the traits and skills in-
volved in formulating such an answer (and in enacting it in the real world) are
things already discussed as major features of social skills or social compe-
tence: interpersonal empathy, adequate self-control, and the ability to ap-
praise a situation in terms of commonly accepted social mores and rules. In
addition to this particular task, several other components of standardized IQ
tests require the ability to make effective social judgments.
However, the inability to measure adequately the practical-social forms of
intelligence has been one of the major criticisms of standardized IQ tests
(Goleman, 1994), and most of the theorizing regarding social-behavioral
skills as a form of practical intelligence has been proposed by those searching
for better alternatives to standardized IQ tests, which tend to differentiate be-
tween academic and social forms of intelligence. This problem was summa-
rized by Mercer, Gomez-Palacio, and Padilla (1986), who stated:
We recognize that there is some overlap (between academic and social forms of
intelligence), especially in infancy, when the two cannot be differentiated em-
pirically. "Intelligence" tests for very young children contain many items relat-
ing to social behaviors, such as smiling, recognizing the mother's face, and rec-
ognizing a stranger. As the child develops, social-behavioral intelligence and
academic intelligence gradually become differentiated, and traditional "intelli-
gence" tests progressively eliminate items involving social competencies as the
SOCIAL SKILLS AS PRACTICAL INTELLIGENCE 25
age of the subject increases. By school age, traditional tests focus almost exclu-
sively on academic intelligence." (p. 308)
The core capacity (of interpersonal intelligence) is the ability to notice and
make distinctions among individuals, and, in particular, among their moods,
temperament, motivations, and intentions. Examined in its most elementary
form, the interpersonal intelligence entails the capacity of the young child to
discriminate among the individuals around him and to detect their various
moods. In an advanced form, interpersonal knowledge permits a skilled adult to
read the intentions and desires-even when these have been hidden-of many
other individuals, and potentially to act upon this knowledge-for example, by
26 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
These are times when the fabric of society seems to unravel at ever-greater speed,
when selfishness, violence, and a meanness of spirit seem to be rotting the good-
ness of our communal lives. Here the argument for the importance of emotional
intelligence hinges on the link between sentiment, character, and moral in-
stincts. There is growing evidence that fundamental ethical stances in life stem
from underlying emotional capacities. For one, impulse is the medium of emo-
tion; the seed of all impulse is a feeling bursting to express itself in action. Those
who are at the mercy of impulse-who lack self-control-suffer a moral defi-
ciency. The ability to control impulse is the base of will and character. By the
same token, the root of altruism lies in empathy, the ability to read emotions in
others; lacking a sense of another's need or despair, there is no caring. And if
there are any two moral stances that our times call for, they are precisely these,
self-restraint and compassion. (p. xii)
We can therefore relate the idea of emotional empathy directly to certain as-
pects of social skills: peer relationships, self-management, and empathy.
SOCIAL SKILLS AS PRACTICAL INTELLIGENCE 27
In summary, at the end of the 20th century there were several prominent
attempt·., to publicly redefine the concept of intelligence or IQ to include and
recognize the importance of social-emotional behavior. In this regard, social
skills may be considered as either a key component or a major mediating vari-
able in intelligence, but most experts in the area of human intelligence are
now at least recognizing their critical importance.
SUMMARY
This chapter introduces the topic of child and adolescent social skills. Chil-
dren and adolescents who have deficits in social skills tend to exhibit these
deficits in differing ways, as is expressed by the three case studies that intro-
duce this chapter.
Like a number of other behavioral or psychological constructs, social skills
have been defined in the professional literature several different ways, and no
single generally agreed upon definition of social skills exists. However, the cur-
rent influential definitions of social skills seem to have some common core
elements, and these definitions thus may be divided into three types: behav-
ioral, peer acceptance, and social validity.
In the literature on assessment and treatment of social skills, specific cate-
gories and subcategories of social skills are identified, and each subcategory
may contain numerous specific component behaviors. Specific examples of
appropriate child and adolescent social skills are generally included in social
skills training programs.
Although the study of children's social behavior has been prominent since
at least the 1930s, only since the 1970s have the fields of psychology and edu-
cation experienced that dramatic upsurge of interest in this area that exists to-
day. Scholarly efforts in the area of child and adolescent social skills since the
1980s have been focused to a great extent on the development of clinically
useful assessment instruments and intervention programs.
Social skills comprise part of a superordinate construct: social competence.
Whereas social skills are generally viewed as being specific behaviors or ac-
tivities that lead to desired social outcomes, social competence is a summary
term based on judgment from others reflecting how a person's implementa-
tion of social skills is viewed in terms of adequacy.
A behavioral dimensions approach to classifying child and adolescent so-
cial skills was presented, wherein the same type of methodology used by Quay
(1986) to develop his influential taxonomy of child and adolescent problem
behaviors was utilized. A taxonomy of five dimensions of social skills resulted
from this meta-analysis: peer relations, self-management, academic, compli-
ance, and assertion skills. These dimensions may have important implications
for future developments in a classification taxonomy for child and adolescent
social skills.
28 NATURE AND VALUE OF SOCIAL SKILLS
Several models picturing the structure of social skills and social compe-
tence have been proposed, and although all of these models view both social
skills and social competence as multidimensional constructs, there are some
differences between these models that are both obvious and subtle. The model
followed generally in this volume considers both social skills and adaptive be-
havior to be components of the superordinate construct of social competence.
Findings show that positive or adequate social skills development during
childhood is an important foundation for good social, occupational, and per-
sonal adjustment throughout life. Conversely, it has been found that inade-
quate social skill development during childhood is associated with a number
of negative outcomes including psychiatric problems; externalizing problems
such as antisocial! aggressive behavior attention-deficit hyperactivity behav-
iors; and internalizing problems such as depression, social withdrawal, and
anxiety. There have been differing perspectives on the causal relationship be-
tween social skills deficits and various social-emotional problems, but the
position emphasized in this volume contends that this relationship is both mu-
tual and reciprocal. In other words, social skills deficits may be caused by var-
ious social-emotional problems, and they may also serve as a causal factor in
the development of such problems.
Several recent conceptualizations of human intelligence have directly or
indirectly acknowledged the importance of social skills for adaptive goal-
oriented behavior. Hence, the concepts of practical intelligence, social intelli-
gence, and interpersonal intelligence have appeared within larger theories of
human intelligence. Some recent widely read books in this area have placed
the importance of social-emotional behavior in the public eye, and it seems
likely that future research and public policy efforts concerning intelligence
and its measurement may include social skills in some important way.
References
Table of Contents
Author Index
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies reported
by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monographs for the
Society for Research in Child De'"velopment, 46 (1 Serial, No. 88).
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Taxonomic issues in child psychopathology. In T. H.
Ollendick & M. Herson (Eds.), Handbook of child psychopathology. New York: Plenum.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1984). Psychopathology of childhood. Annual Review of
Psychology, 35,227-256.
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and
emotional problems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity.
Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213-232.
Adalbjarnardottir, S. (1993). Promoting children's social growth in the schools: An intervention
study. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 14, 461-484.
Akhtar, N., & Bradley, E. J. (1991). Social information processing deficits of aggressive children:
Present findings and implications for social skills training. Clinical Psychology Review, 11,
621-644.
Albano, M., Marten, P. A., Holt, C. S., Heimberg, R. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Cognitive-behav-
ioral group treatment for social phobia in adolescents: A preliminary study. Journal of Ner-
vous and Mental Disease, 183, 649-656.
Alessi, G. (1988). Direct observation methods for emotional/behavior problems. In E. S. Shapiro
& T. R. Kratochwill (Eds.), Behavioral assessment in schools: Conceptual/oundations and
practical applications (pp. 14-75). New York: Guilford.
American Association on Mental Retardation (1992)'lvlental retardation: De/inition, classifica-
tion, and systems of support (9th ed.). Washington. DC: American Association on Mental Re-
tardation.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual 0/ mental disor-
ders (4th cd.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Argyle, M. (1981). Social skills and health. London: Metheun.
182
REFERENCES 183
Arkcs. H. R. (1981). Impediments to accurate clinical judgment and possible ways to minimize
their impact. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 323-330.
Asher, S. R., Oden, S., & Gottman, J. (1977). Children's friendships in school settings. In L. Katz,
M. Glockner, S. Goodman, & N. Spencer (Eds.), Current topics in early childhood education.
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Asher, S. R., & Parker, J. G. (1989). Significance of peer relationship problems in childhood. In
B. H. Schneider, G. Attili, J. Nadel, & R. P. Weissberg (Eds.), Social competence in develop-
mental perspecticoe (pp. 5-23). Boston: Kluwer.
Asher. S. R., & Taylor, A. R. (1981). The social outcomes of mainstreaming: Sociometric assess-
ment and beyond. Exceptional Children Quarterly, 1, 13-30.
Asher, S. R., & Wheeler, V. A. (1985). Children's loneliness: A comparison of rejected and ne-
glected peer status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 500-505.
i\.,elson, J. A. (1993). Counseling and development in a multicultural society (2nd cd.). Pacific
Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33,
344-358.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.
Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and
treatment. New York: Guilford.
Barnett, D. W., & Zucker, K. B. (1990). The personal and social assessment of children. Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Barton, E. J., & Ascione, F. R. (1984). Direct observation. In T. H. Ollendick & M. Herson (Eds.),
Child behavioral assessment: Principles and procedures (pp. 166-194). New York: Pergam-
mon Press.
Bell, N. J., & Carver, W. A. (1980). A reevaluation of gender label effects: Expectant mothers' re-
sponses to infants. Child Decoelopment, 51, 925-927.
Bell-Dolan, D. J., Reaven, N. M., & Peterson, 1. (1993). Depression and social functioning: A multi-
dimensional study of the linkages. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 306-315.
Bierman, K. L., & Furman, W. (1984). The effects of social skills training and peer involvement
on the social adjustment of preadolescents. Child Development, 55, 151-162.
Bierman, K. 1., Miller, C. 1., & Stabb, S. D. (1987). Improving the social behavior and peer ac-
ceptance of rejected boys: Effects of social skills training with instructions and prohibitions.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 194-200.
Bloom, B. 1. (1968). An ecological analysis of psychiatric hospitalization. Multicoariate Behm.J-
ioral Research, 3, 423-463.
Boggs, S. R., & Eyberg, S. (1990). Interview techniques and establishing rapport. In 1. M. La-
Greca (Ed.), Through the eyes of the child (pp. 85-108). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Bower, E. (1969). Early identtfication of emotionally handicapped children in school (2nd ed.).
Springfield, IL: Thomas.
Bracken, B. A. (1992). Multidimensional Self-Concept Scale. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Bracken, B. A. (1993). Assessment of Interpersonal Relations. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Bracken, B. A., Keith, 1. K., & Walker, K. C. (1994). Assessment of preschool behavior and social-
emotional functioning: A review of thirteen third-party instruments. Assessment in Rehabili-
tation and Exceptionality, 1, 331-346.
Brinton, B., & Fujiki, M. (1982). A comparison of request-response sequences in the discourse of
normal and language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 47, 57-
62.
Broekhoff, J. (1977). A search for relationships: Sociological and social-psychological considera-
tions. The Academy Papers, 11, 45-55.
184 REFERENCES
Bruininks, R. H., Woodcock, R. W., Weathennan, R. F., & Hill, B. K. (1984). Scales of Indepen-
dent Behavior. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.
Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and language acquisition. In H. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies
in mother-infant interaction (pp. 271-289). London: Academic Press.
Bryan, T., Pearl, R., Donahue, M., & Pt1aum, T. (1983). The Chicago Institute for the Study of
Learning Disabilities. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 4, 1-23.
Bryant, B., Trower, P., Yardley, K., Urbieta, H., & Letemendia, F. T. (1976). A survey of social in-
adequacy among psychiatric outpatients. Psychological Medicine, 6, 101-112.
Bulkeley, R., & Cramer, D. (1994). Social skills training with young adolescents: Group and indi-
vidual approaches in a school setting. Journal of Adolescence, 17, 521-531.
Butler, L., Mietzitis, S., Friedman, R., & Cole, E. (1980). The effect of two school-based interven-
tion programs on depressive symptoms in preadolescents. American Educational Research
Journal, 17,111-119.
Caldarella, P., & Merrell, K. W. (1997). Common dimensions of social skills of children and ado-
lescents: A taxonomy of positive behaviors. School Psychology ReView, 26, 265-279.
Cartledge, G., & Milburn, J. F. (1986). Selecting social skills. In G. Cartledge & J. F. Milburn
(Eds.), Teaching social skills to children (2nd ed.) (pp. 7-28). New York: Pergamon Press.
Cartledge, G., & Milburn, J. F. (1995). Teaching social skills to children and youth: Innovative
approaches (3rd ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Center, D. B., & Wascom, A. M. (1986). Teacher perceptions of social behavior in learning dis-
abled and normal children and youth. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19,420-425.
Chandler, L. K., Lubeck, R. C., & Fowler, S. A. (1992). Generalization and maintenance of pre-
school children's social skills: A critical review and analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 25, 415-428.
Chin-Perez, G., Hartman, D., Park, H. S., & Sacks, S. L. (1986). Maximizing social contact for sec-
ondary students with severe handicaps. Journal of the Association for Persons with Se'"vere
Handicaps, 11, 118-124.
Clarke, G., Lewinsohn, P., & Hops, H. (1990). The adolescent coping with depression course. Eu-
gene, OR: Castalia.
Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Cappotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-
age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 557-570.
Cole, D. A., & Carpentieri, S. (1990). Social status and the comorbidity of child depression and
conduct disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58,748-757.
Combs, M. L., & Slaby, O. A. (1977). Social skills training with children. In B. B. Lahey & A. E.
Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 161-201). New York: Plenum.
Conger, J. C., & Keane, S. P. (1981). Social skills intervention in the treatment of isolated or with-
drawn children. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 478-495.
Connolly, T., Dowd, T., Criste, A., Nelson, C., & Tobias, L. (1995). The well-managed classroom:
Promoting success through social skill instnlCtion. Boys Town, NE: Boys Town Press.
Cowen, E. L., Pederson, A., Babigan, H., Izzo, L. D., & Trost, M. A. (1973). Long-term follow-up
of early detected vulnerable children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 41,
438-446.
Craig, H., & Evans, J. (1989). Turn exchange characteristics of SLI children's simultaneous and
non-simultaneous speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 334-337.
Craig, H., & Gallagher, T. (1986). Interactive play: The frequency of related verbal responses.
Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 62, 474-482.
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing
mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74-101.
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's aggressive behavior. Child De'"ve/opment, 51,
162-170.
REFERENCES 185
Dodge, K. A. (1985). Facets of social interaction and the assessment of social competence in chil-
dren. In B. H. Schneider, K. H. Rubin, & J. E. Ledingham (Eds.), Children's peer relations:
Issues in assessment and inte1""eJention (pp. 3-22). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Dodge, K. A., & Frame, C. L. (1982). Social cognitive biases and deficits in aggressive boys. Child
DeceJelopment, 53, 620-635.
Doll, E. A. (1935). A genetic scale of social maturity. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
5,180-188.
Dowd, T., & Tierney, J. (1992). Teaching social skills to youth: A curriculum for child-care
proceJiders. Boys Town, NE: Boys Town Press.
Dujovne, V. F., Barnard, M. U., & Rapoff, M. A. (1995). Pharmacological and cognitive-behav-
ioral approaches in the treatment of childhood depression: A review and critique. Clinical
Psychology Re'View, 15, 589-611.
DuPaul, G. J .. & Eckert, T. L. (1994). The effects of social skills curricula: Now you see them, now
you don't. School Psychology Quarterly, 9, 113-132.
DuPaul, G. J., & Stoner, G. (1994). ADHD in the schools: Assessment and inte1""eJention strate-
gies. New York: Guilford.
Dygdon, J. A. (1993). The culture and lifestyle appropriate social skills curriculum: A program
for socially 'Valid social skills training. New York: Wiley.
Edleson, J. L., & Rose, S. D. (1982). Investigations into the efficacy of short-term group social
skills training for socially isolated children. Child Behavior Therapy, 3(2/3), 1-16.
Eisenberg, N. (1983). Sex-typed toy choices: What to they signify? In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and
cogniti'Ve skills: Sex roles and children's play (pp. 45-70). New York: Academic Press.
Eisenberg, N., & Harris, J. D. (1984). Social competence: A developmental perspective. School
Psychology RcceJiew, 13, 267-277.
Eisenberg, N., Murray, E., & Hite, T. (1982). Children's reasoning regarding sex-typed toy
choices. Child DeceJelopment, 53, 81-86.
Elksnin, L. K., & Elksnin, N. (1995). Assessment and instruction of social skills. San Diego, CA:
Singular Publishing Group.
Elliott, S. N., & Gresham, F. M. (1991). Social skin~ inte1""eJention guide: Practical strategies for
social skills training. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance.
Emerson, E. N., Crowley, S. L., & Merrell, K. W. (1994). Convergent validity of the School Social
Behavior Scales with the Child Behavior Checklist and Teacher's Report Form. Journal of
PsychoeducationalAssessment, 12, 372-380.
Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Erwin, P. G. (1994). Effectiveness of social skills training with children: A meta-analytic study.
Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 7,305-310.
Etaugh, C. (1983). Introduction: The influence of environmental factors on sex differences in
children's play. In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles and children's play
(pp. 1-19). New York: Academic Press.
Fagot, B. l. (1974). Reinforcing contingencies for sex-role behaviors: Effect of experience with
children. Child DcceJelopment, 49, 30-36.
Fagot, B. I. (1981). Male and female teachers: Do they treat boys and girls differently? Sex Roles,
7,263-271.
Fagot, B. I., & Patterson, G. R. (1969). In-vivo analysis of reinforcing contingencies for sex-role
behaviors in the preschool. Developmental Psychology, 1, 563-568.
Feshbach, N. D. (1983). Learning to care: A positive approach to child training and discipline.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 266-271.
Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach, S. (1982). Empathy training and the regulation of aggression: Po-
tentialities and limitations. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 4, 399-413.
186 REFERENCES
Foster, S. L., & Ritchey, W. L. (1979). Issues in the assessment of social competence in children.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 625-638.
Fox, J. L., & McEvoy, M. A. (1993). Assessing and enhancing generalization and social validity of
social skills interventions with children and adolescents. Behavior Modification, 17, 339-
366.
Frederick, B. P., & Olmi, D. J. (1994). Children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A
review of the literature on social skills deficits. Psychology in the Schools, 31, 288-296.
Fuchs, D., & Fuchs, L. S. (1994). Inclusive schools movement and the radicalization of special ed-
ucation reform. Exceptional Children, 60, 294-309.
Furman, W., & Bierman, K. L. (1982). Developmental changes in young children's conceptions
of friendship. Child De'"velopment, 54, 549-556.
Gallagher, T., & Craig, H. (1984). Pragmatic assessment: Analysis of a highly frequent repeated
utterance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49, 368-377.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Books.
Gesten, E. L. (1976). A health resources inventory: The development of a measure of the personal
and social competence of primary-grade children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
chology, 44, 775-786.
Goldberg, D., & Huxley, P. (1980). Mental illness in the community. London: Tavistock Publica-
tions.
Goldstein, A. P. (1988). The prepare curriculum: teaching prosocial competencies. Champaign,
IL: Research Press.
Goldstein, A. P., Glick, B., Reiner, S., Zimmerman, D., & Coultry, T. M. (1987). Aggression re-
placement training: A comprehensive inteFventionfor aggreSSive youth. Champaign, IL: Re-
search Press.
Goldstein, A. P., & Pentz, M. A. (1984). Psychological skill training and the aggressive adolescent.
School Psychology Re'"view, 13, 311-323.
Goldstein, A. P., Sprafkin, R. P., Gershaw, N. J., & Klein, P. (1980). Skillstreaming the adolescent.
Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Goldstein, H., & Gallagher, T. M. (1992). Strategies for promoting the social-communicative com-
petence of young children with specific language impairment. In S. R. Odom, S. L. McCon-
nell, & M. A. McEvoy (eds.), Social competence of young children with disabilities (pp. 189-
213). Baltimore: Brookes.
Goleman, D. (1994). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam.
Gottlieb, J. (1975). Public, peer, and professional attitudes toward mentally retarded persons. In
M. J. Bagab & S. A. Richardson (Eds.), The mentally retarded and society: A social science
perspective (pp. 99-125). Baltimore: University Park Press.
Gottlieb, J. (1981). Mainstreaming: Fulfilling the promise? American Journal on Mental Defi-
ciency, 86, 115-126.
Gottman, J. M. (1983). How children become friends. Monograph" of the Society for Research
in Child Development, 48 (3, Serial No. 201).
Gottman, J. M. (1986). The observation of social process. In J. M. Gottman &J. C. Parker (Eds.),
Conversations of friends: Speculation on affective development (pp. 51-102). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Grenell, M. M., Glass, C. R., & Katz, K. S. (1987). Hyperactive children and peer interaction:
Knowledge and performance of social skills. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 15, 1-13.
Gresham, F. M. (1981). Social skills training with handicapped children. A review. Re'"view of Ed-
ucationalResearch, 51,139-176.
Gresham, F. M. (1982). Misguided mainstreaming: The case for social skills training with handi-
capped children. Exceptional Children, 48, 422-433.
Gresham, F. M. (1986). Conceptual issues in the assessment of social competence in children. In
REFERENCES 187
Hops, H. (1983). Children's social competence and skill: Current research practices and future
directions. Behavior Therapy, 14, 3-18.
Hops, H., & Finch, M. (1985). Social competence and skills: A reassessment. In B. H. Schneider,
K. H. Rubin, and J. E. Ledingham (Eds.), Children's peer relations: Issues in assessment and
inter"vention (pp. 23-39). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Horney, K. (1945). Our inner conflicts. New York: Norton.
Howell, K. W. (1985). A task-analytical approach to social behavior. Remedial and Special Edu-
cation, 6(2), 24-30.
Huggins, P. (1990-1994). Teaching cooperation skills. Longmont, CO: Sopris-West.
Huggins, P. (1993-1995). Teachingfriendship skills: Primary version. Longmont, CO: Sopris-
West.
Hughes, J. N., & Baker, D. B. (1990). The clinical child inter"view. New York: Guilford.
Hughes, J. N., & Sullivan, K. A. (1988). Outcome assessment in social skills training with chil-
dren. Journal of School Psychology, 26, 167-183.
Hundert, J., & Houghton, A. (1992). Promoting social interaction of children with disabilities in
integrated preschools: A failure to generalize. Exceptional Children, 58, 311-320.
Interagency Committee on Learning Disabilities (1987). Learning disabilities: A report to the
U. S. Congress. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health.
Jackson, N. F., Jackson, D. A., & Monroe, C. (1983). Getting along with others: Teaching social
effectiveness to children. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Jentzsch, C. E. (1993). The predictive validity of the Battelle Developmental Inventory as a
measure of social-behavioral dccoelopmentforyoung children with disabilities. Unpublished
Masters thesis, Utah State University. Logan, UT.
Jentzsch, C. E., & Merrell, K. W. (1996). An investigation of the construct validity of the
Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales. Diagnostique, 21(2), 1-15.
Johnson, J. E., & Roopnarine, J. L. (1983). The preschool classroom and sex differences in chil-
dren's play. In M. B. Liss (Ed.), Social and cognitive skills: Sex roles and children's play (pp.
193-218). New York: Academic Press.
Jones, R. N., Sheridan, S. M., & Binns, W. R. (1993). Schoolwide social skills training: Providing
preventative services to children at-risk. School Psychology Quarterly, 8, 57-80.
Kamps, D. M., Leonard, B. R., Vernon, S., & Dugan, E. P. (1992). Teaching social skills to students
with autism to increase peer interactions in an integrated first-grade classroom. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 281-288.
Kauffman, J. M. (1989). Characteristics of behavior disorders of children and youth (4th ed.).
Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Kauffman, J. M. (1993). Special problems in the inclusion of students with emotional or behav-
ioral disorders in general education classrooms and schools. Special Education Perspectieves,
2(1),23-28.
Kauffman, J. M. (1995). How we might achieve the radical reform of special education. In J. M.
Kauffman & D. P. Hallahan (Eds.), The illusion of full inclusion (pp. 193-211). Austin, TX:
Pro-Ed.
Kauffman, J. M., Semmell, M. I., & Agard, J. A. (1974). PRIME: An overview. Education and
Training for the Mentally Retarded, 9, 107-112.
Kazdin, A. E. (1981). Behavioral observation. In M. Herson &A. S. Bellack (Eds.), Behavioral as-
sessment: A practical handbook (pp. 101-124). New York: Pergamon.
Kazdin, A. E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods for clinical and applied settings.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Kazdin, A. E. (1987). Treatment of antisocial behavior in children: Current status and future di-
rections. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 187-203.
Kazdin, A. E., Esveldt-Dawson, K., French, N. H., & Unis, A. S. (1987). Problem-solving skills
REFERENCES 189
training and relationship therapy in the treatment of antisocial child behavior. Journal of Con-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 76-85.
Keane, S. P., & Conger, J. C. (1981). The implications of communication development for social
skills training. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 6, 369-381.
Keller, H. R. (1986). Behavioral observation approaches to assessment. In H. Knoff (Ed.), The as-
sessment of child and adolescent personality (pp. 353-397). New York: Gilford.
Kelly, J. A. (1982). Social skills training: A practical gUide for inter-"entions. New York:
Springer.
Kettlewell, P. W., & Kausch, D. F. (1983). The generalization of the effects of a cognitive-behav-
ioral treatment program for aggressive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11,
101-114.
Knapczyk, D. R., & Rodes, P. G. (1996). Teaching social competence. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/
Cole.
Knoff, H. M., & Batsche, G. M. (1995). Projects ACHIEVE: Analyzing a school reform process for
at-risk and underachieving students. School Psychology Rec"iew, 24, 579-603.
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socializa-
tion. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand-
McNally.
Kolko, D. J., Loar, L. L., & Sturnick, D. (1990). Inpatient social-cognitive skills training groups
with conduct disordered and attention deficit disordered children. Journal of Child Psychol-
ogy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 31,737-748.
Kratochwill, T. R., & French, D. C. (1984). Social skills training for withdrawn children. School
Psychology Rec"iew, 13, 331-338.
LaGreca, A. M., & Vaughn, S. (1992). Social functioning of individuals with learning disabilities.
School Psychology Re-view, 21, 340-347.
Lambert, N., & Windmiller, N. (1981). AMID Adaptive Behavior Scale-School Edition. Mon-
terey, CA: Publishers Test Service.
Landau, S., & Milich, R. (1988). Social communication patterns of attention-deficit-disordered
boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 69-81.
Landau, S., & Milich, R. (1990). Assessment of children's social status and peer relations. InA. M.
LaGreca (Ed.), Through the eyes of the child (pp. 259-291). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Landau, S., & Moore, L. A. (1991). Social skills deficits in children with attention-deficit hyper-
activity disorder. School Psychology Rcc"iew, 20, 235-251.
Law, J., Brown, G., & Lester, R. (1994). Speech and language therapy provision in a social edu-
cation centre: The value of a "first step" assessment. British Journal of Learning Disabilities,
22,66-69.
Lethermon, V. R., Williamson, D. A., Moody, S. C., Granberry, S. W., Lenauer, K. L., & Bodiford,
C. B. (1984). Factors affecting the social validity of a role-play test of children's social skills.
Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 6, 231-245.
Lethermon, V. R., Williamson, D. A., Moody, S. C., & Wozniak, P. (1986). Racial bias in behav-
ioral assessment of children's social skills. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral As-
sessment, 8, 329-337.
Lewinsohn, P. (1974). A behavioral approach to depression. In R. Friedman &M. Katz (Eds.), The
psychology of depression: Contemporary theory and research. Washington, DC: U. S. Gov-
ernment Printing Office.
Lewinsohn, P. M., Clarke, G. N., Hops, H., & Andrews, J. A. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral treat-
ment for depressed adolescents. Behavior Therapy, 21, 385-401.
Libet, J., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (1973). The concept of social skill with special references to the be-
havior of depressed persons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 304-312.
190 REFERENCES
Lochman, J. E. (1992). Cognitive-behavioral intervention with aggressive boys: Three year follow-
up and preventative effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 426-432.
Lochman, J. E., Burch, P. R., Curry, J. F., & Lampron, L. B. (1984). Treatment and generalization
effects of cognitive-behavioral and goal-setting interventions with aggressive boys. Jounwl of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 915-916.
Loeber, R., Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (1984). Multiple gating: A multistage assessment
procedure for identifying youths at risk for delinquency. Journal of Research in Crime and
Delinquency, 21,7-32.
Lovejoy, M. C., & Routh, D. K. (1988). Behavior disordered children's social skills: Increased by
training, but not sustained or reciprocated. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 10, 15-27.
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stan-
ford University Press.
MacMillan, D. L., Gresham, F. M., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Full inclusion: An empirical perspec-
tive. Behavioral Disorders, 21, 145-159.
Martin, R. P. (1988). Assessment of personality and behavior problems. New York: Guilford.
Martin, R. P., Hooper, S., & Snow, J. (1986). Behavior rating scale approaches to personality as-
sessment in children and adolescents. In H. Knoff (Ed.), The assessment of child and ado-
lescent personality (pp. 309-351). New York: Guilford.
Masten, A. S., Morrison, P., & Pelligrini, D. S. (1985). A rcvised class play method of peer assess-
ment. Developmental Psychology, 21,523-533.
Mathews, W. S. (1977). Modes of transformation in the initiation of fantasy play. Developmental
Psychology, 13, 212-216.
Matson, J. L. (1984). Issues in assessing social skills deficits and excesses in handicapped chil-
dren. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Dec"elopmental Disabilities, 10, 203-207.
Matson, J. L. (1988). Teaching and training relevant community skills to mentally retarded per-
sons. Child and Youth SeFoices, 10, 107-121.
Matson, J. L., Compton, L. S., & Sevin, J. A. (1991). Comparison and item analysis of the MESSY
for autistic and normal children. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 361-369.
Matson, J. L., Esveldt-Dawson, K., & Kazdin, A. E. (1983). Validation of methods for assessing so-
cial skills in children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 174-180.
Matson, J. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (1988). EnhanCing children's social skills. New York: Pergamon.
Matson, J. L., Rotatori, A. F., & Helsel, W. J. (1983). Development of a rating scale to measure so-
cial skills in children: The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters (MESSY). Be-
havior Research and Therapy, 21, 335-340.
McCandless, B., & Marshall, H. (1957). A picture sociometric technique for preschool children
and its relation to teacher judgments of friendship. Child Dccoelopmene, 28, 139-148.
McCarver, R. B., & Campbell, V. A. (1987). Future developments in the concept and application
of adaptive behavior. The Journal of Special Education, 21, 197-207.
McFall, R. M. (1982). A review and reformulation of the construct of social skills. Belwc"ioraIAs-
sessment, 4, 1-33.
McGinnis, E., & Goldstein, A. P. (1984). Skillstreaming for the elementary-age child. Cham-
paign, IL: Research Press.
McGinnis, E., & Goldstein, A. P. (1990). Skillstreaming in early childhood: Teaching prosocial
skilL~ to the preschool and kindergarten child. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
McGuire, J., & Priestley, P. (1981). Life after school: A social skills curriculum. Oxford: Perga-
mon.
McLoyd, V. C. (1980). Verbally expressed modes of transformation in the fantasy play of black
preschool children. Child Development, 51, 1133-1139.
McMahon, C. M.. Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G. M., & Melloy. K. J. (1994). Evaluation of the multiple
effects of a social skill intervention. Behacoioral Disorders, 20, 35-50.
Meehl, P. (1954). Clinical 'Os. statistical prediction. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
REFERENCES 191
Meichenbaum, D., Butler, L., & Gruson, L. (1981). Toward a conceptual model of social compe-
tence. In J. D. Wyne & M. D. Smye (Eds.), Social competence. New York: Guilford.
Mercer, C. D., Hughes, C., & Mercer, A. R. (1985). Learning disability definitions used by state
education departments. Learning Disability Quarterly, 8, 45-55.
Mercer, J. R., Gomez-Palacio, M., & Padilla, E. (1986). The development of practical intelligence
in cross-cultural perspective. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (Eds.), Practical intelligence
(pp. 307-337). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Merrell, K. W. (1989). Concurrent relationships between two behavior rating scales for teachers:
An examination of self-control, social competence, and school behavioral adjustment. Psy-
chology in the Schools, 26, 267-271.
Merrell, K. W. (1991). Teacher ratings of social competence and behavioral adjustment: Differ-
ences between learning disabled, low-achieving, and typical students. Journal of School Psy-
chology, 29, 207-217.
Merrell, K. W. (1993a). Using behavior rating scales to assess social skills and antisocial behavior
in school settings: Development of the school social behavior scales. School Psychology Re-
c"iew, 22, 115-133.
Merrell, K. W. (1993b). School Social Behavior Scales. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Merrell, K. W. (1994a). Assessment of behaVioral, social, and emotional problems: Direct and
objective methods for use with children and adolescents. White Plains, NY: Longman.
Merrell, K. W. (1994b). Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Merrell, K. W. (1995a). An investigation of the relationship between social skills and internalizing
problems in early childhood: Construct validity of the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior
Scales. Jounwl of Psychoeducational Assessment, 13, 230-240.
Merrell, K. W. (1995b). Relationships among early childhood behavior rating scales: Convergent
and discriminant construct validity of the Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales. Early
Education and Development, 6, 253-264.
Merrell, K. W. (1996). Assessment of social skills and behavior problems in early childhood: The
Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales. Journal of Early Intervention, 20, 132-145.
Merrell, K. W., & Gill, S. J. (1994). Using teacher ratings of social behavior to differentiate gifted
from non-gifted students. Roeper Rcc"iew, 16(4), 286-289.
Merrell, K. W., Merz, J. M., Johnson, E. R., & Ring, E. N. (1992). Social competence of students
with mild handicaps and low achievement: A comparative study. School Psychology Review,
21,125-137.
Merrell, K. W., Sanders, D. E., & Popinga, M. (1993). Teacher ratings of social behavior as a pre-
dictor of special education status: Discriminant validity of the School Social Behavior Scales.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 11, 220-231.
Merrell, K. W., & Shinn, M. R. (1990). Critical variables in the learning disabilities identification
process. School Psychology Review, 19, 74-82.
Michelson, L., Sugai, D., Wood, R., & Kazdin, A. E. (1983). Social skills assessment and train-
ing with children: An empirical handbook. New York: Plenum.
Milich, R., & Landau, S. (1984). A comparison of the social status and social behavior of aggres-
sive and aggressive/withdrawn boys. Journal of Abnonnal Child Psychology, 12, 277-288.
Moore, L. A. (1994). The effects of social skills curricula: Were they apparent initially? School
Psychology Quarterly, 9, 133-136.
Moreno, J. L. (1934). Who shall sUT""ive? Washington, DC: Nervous and Mental Disease Publish-
ing.
Muscott, H. S., & Gifford, T. (1994). Virtual reality and social skills training for students with be-
havior disorders: Applications, challenges and promising practices. Education and Treat-
ment of Children, 1 7, 417 -434.
Mussen, P. H. (Ed.). (1970). Carmichael's manual of child psychology (Vol. 3). New York: Wiley.
192 REFERENCES
National Institutes of Health. (1990). Epidemiology of specific language impairment. RFP NIH-
DC-90~19. Washington, DC: Author.
Nientemp, E. G., & Cole, C. L. (1992). Teaching socially valid interaction responses to students
with sevcre disabilities in an integrated school sctting. Joumal of School Psychology, 30,
343~354.
O'Donnell, J., Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F., Abbott, R. D., & Day, L. E. (1995). Preventing
school failure, drug use, and delinquency among low-income children: Long-term interven-
tion in elementary schools. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 87~ 100.
Ogilvy, C. M. (1994). Social skills training with children and adolescents: A review of the evidence
of effectiveness. Educational Psychology, 14, 73~83.
O'Reilly, J. P., Tokuno, K. A., & Ebata, A. T. (1986). Cultural differences between Americans of
Japanese and European ancestry in parental valuing of social competence. Joumal of Com-
parative Family Studies, 17, 87~97.
Parke, H. S., Tappe, P., Carmeto, R., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1990). Social support and quality of life
for learning disabled and mildly retarded youth in transition. In R. Gaylord-Ross, S. Siegel, H.
S. Park, S. Sacks, & L. Goetz (Eds.), Readings in ecosocial de-ve/opment (pp. 293~328). San
Francisco: San Francisco State University, Department of Special Education.
Parker, J., & Gottman, J. (1989). Social and emotional development in relational context. In
T. Berndt & G. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child development (pp. 95~131). New York:
Wiley.
Peacock Hill Working Group. (1991). Problems and promises in special education and related ser-
vices for children and youth with emotional or behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders,
16, 299~313.
Pelligrini, D. (1980). Social cognition, competence, and adaptation in children under stress. In
N. Gamezy (Ed.), Studies of stress and coping in children. Symposium conducted at the
meeting of the American Psychological Association, Montreal, Canada.
Pepler, D. J., King, G., Craig, W., Byrd, B., & Bream, L. (1995). The development and evaluation
of a multisystem social skills group training program for aggressive children. Child and Youth
Care Forum, 24, 297~313.
Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 1).
New York: Wiley.
Polyson, J., & Kimball, W. (1993). Social skills training with physically aggressive children. In
A. J. Finch, W. M. Nelson, & E. S. Ott (Eds.), Cognitive-behw"ioral procedures with children
and adolescents: A practical gUide (pp. 206~232). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Quay, H. C. (1986). Classification. In H. C. Quay & J. S. Werry (Eds.), Psychological disorders
of childhood (3rd ed., pp. 1~34). New York: Wiley.
Reed, M. K. (1994). Social skill training to reduce depression in adolescents. Adolescence, 29,
293~302.
Reid, J. B. (1982). Observer training in naturalistic research. In D. P. Hartmann (Ed.), Using ob-
seF"ers to study behavior (pp. 37~50). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Reid, J. B., Baldwin, D. B., Patterson, G. R., & Dishion, T. J. (1988). Observations in the assess-
ment of childhood disorders. In M. Rutter, A. H. Tuma, & I. S. Lann (Eds.), Assessment and
diagnosis in child psychopathology (pp. 156~195). New York: Guilford.
Reschly. D. J., & Gresham, F. M. (1981). Use of social competence measures tofacilitate parent
and teacher iwvolvement and nonbiased (~5Sessment. Unpublished manuscript, Iowa State
University, Ames.
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (1992). The Behavioral Assessment Systemfor Children.
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.
Rhode, G., Jenson, W. R., & Reavis, H. K. (1992). The tough kid book: Practical classroom man-
agement strategies. Longmont, CO: Sopris-West.
REFERENCES 193
Rime, B" Bouvy, H" Leborgne, B., & Rouillon, F. (1978). Psychopathy and non-verbal behaviour
in an interpersonal situation. Journal oj Abnormal Psychology, 87, 636-643.
Roff, M. (1961). Childhood social interactions and young adult bad conduct. Journal oj Abnor-
mal and Social Psychology, 63,333-337.
Roff, M., Sells, B., & Golden, M. M. (1972). Social adjustment and personality dec"elopment in
children. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Rourke, B. P., & Fuerst, D. R. (1992). Psychosocial dimensions of learning disability subtypes:
Neuropsychological studies in the Windsor Laboratory. School Psychology Review, 21, 361-
374.
Rubin, K. H., & Ross, H. S. (1982). Some reflections on the state of the art: The study of peer re-
lationships and social skills. In K. H. Rubin & H. S. Ross (Eds.), Peerrelatiollships and social
skills in childhood (pp. 1-8). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Rule, S .. Stowitschek, J. J., Innocenti, M., & Striefel, S. (1987). The Social Integration Program:
An analysis of the effects of main streaming handicapped children into day care centers. Edu-
cation and Treatment oj Children, 10, 175-192.
Rutherford, R., Chipman, J., DiGangi, S., & Anderson, K. (1992). Teaching social skills: A prac-
tical instructional approach. Ann Arbor, MI: Exceptional Innovations.
Sargent, L. R. (1991). Social skills jor school and community: Systematic instruction jor chil-
dren and youth with cognitic"e delays. Reston, VA: Division on Mental Retardation, Council
for Exceptional Children.
Sattler, J. M. (1988). Assessment oj children (3rd ed.). San Diego: Jerome M. Sattler.
Schlundt, D., & McFall, R. (1985). New directions in the assessment of social competence and
skills. In L. L' Aabate & M. Milan (Eds.), Handbook oj social skills training and research. New
York: Wiley.
Schneider, B. H. (1992). Didactic methods for enhancing children's peer relations: A quantitative
review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 363-382.
Schneider, B. H., Rubin, K. H., & Ledingham, J. (1985). Children's peer relations: Issues in as-
sessment and inteF"ention. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Schumaker, J., Deshler, D., Alley, G., & Warner, M. (1983). Toward the development of an inter-
vention model for learning-disabled adolescents: The University of Kansas Institute for Re-
search on Learning Disabilities. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 4, 44-47.
Selman, R. L. (1981). The child as a friendship philosopher. In S. R. Asher & J. M. Gottman
(Eds.), The dcc"elopment ojchildren'sjriendship (pp. 242-272). Cambridge, England: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Shapiro, E. S., & Skinner, C. H. (1990). Best practices in observation and ecological assessment.
In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology-II (pp. 507-518).
Washington, DC: National Association of School Psychologists.
Sheinker, J., & Sheinker, A. (1988). Metacognitive approach to social skills tmining: A program
jor grades 4-12. Frederick, MD: Aspen.
Sheridan, S. M. (1995). The tough kid social skills book. Longmont, CO: Sopris West.
Sheridan, S. M., Dee, C. C., Morgan, J. C., McCormick, M. E., & Walker, D. (1996). A multi-
method intervention for social skills deficits in children with ADHD and their parents. School
Psychology Review, 25,57-76.
Shure, M., & Spivack, G. (1980). Interpersonal problem solving as a mediator of behavioral ad-
justment in preschool and kindergarten children. Journal oj Applied Developmental Psy-
chology, 1,29-44.
Snell, M. E. (1991). Schools are for all kids: The importance of integration for students with se-
vere disabilities and their peers. In J. W. Lloyd, A. C. Repp, & N. N. Singh (Eds.), The Regular
Education Initiative: Alternative perspectives on concepts, issues, and models (pp. 133-
148). Sycamore, IL: Sycamore.
194 REFERENCES
Stainback, S., & Stainback, W. (1992). Schools as inclusive communities. In W. Stainback &
S. Stainback (Eds.), Controversial issues confronting special education (pp. 29-43). Bos-
ton: Allyn & Bacon.
Stainback, W., & Stainback, S. (1984). A rationale for the merger of special and regular educa-
tion. Exceptional Children, 51, 102-11l.
Stark, K. D. (1990). Childhood depression: School-ba..~ed inteFventions. New York: Guilford.
Stark, K. D., Best, L. R., & Sellstrom, E. A. (1989). A cognitive-behavioral approach to the treat-
ment of childhood depression. In J. N. Hughes & R. J. Hall (Eds.), Cognitive behavioral psy-
chology in the schools: A comprehensive handbook (pp. 389-433). New York: Guilford.
Stark, K. D., Reynolds, W. M., & Kaslow, N. J. (1987). A comparison of the relative efficacy of self-
control therapy and a behavioral problem-solving therapy for depression in children. Journal
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 15, 91-113.
Stephens, T. M. (1978). Social skills in the classroom. Columbus, OH: Cedars Press.
Stern, J. B., & Fodor, I. G. (1989). Anger control in children: A review of social skills and cogni-
tive behavioral approaches to dealing with aggressive children. Child and Family Behacvior
Therapy, 11, 1-20.
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). The nature and scope of practical intelligence. In R. J. Sterberg & R. A.
Wagner (Eds.), Practical Intelligence (pp. 1-12). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
Stewart, S. L., & Rubin, K. H. (1995). The social problem-solving skills of anxious-withdrawn chil-
dren. Dcc"elopment and Psychopathology, 7, 323-336.
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). In implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied
Behavior AnalYSiS, 19, 349-367.
Strain, P. S. (1983). Identification of social skill curriculum targets for severely handicapped chil-
dren in mainstream preschools. Applied Research in Mental Retardation, 4, 369-382.
Stumme, V. S., Gresham, F. M., & Scott, N. A. (1982). Validity of social behavior assessment in
discriminating emotionally disturbed and nonhandicapped students. Journal of Behavioral
Assessment, 4, 327-341.
Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (1990). Counseling the culturally different (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Swanson, H. L., & Malone, S. (1992). Social skills and learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of the
literature. School Psychology Rccview, 21, 427-443.
Thorndike, E. L. (1920). Intelligence and its uses. Harpers Magazine, 140, 227-235.
Trower, P. M. (1979). Fundamentals of interpersonal behavior: A social-psychological perspec-
tive. In A. S. Bellack & M. Herson (Eds.), Research and practice in social skills training. New
York: Plenum.
Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., Herson, M., & Bellack, A. S. (1984). Effects of race on ratings of so-
cial skill. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 474-475.
Ullman, C. A. (1975). Teachers, peers, and tests as predictors of adjustment. Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 48, 257-267.
Vaughn, S. (1987). TLC-Teaching, learning, and caring: Teaching interpersonal problem-solv-
ing skills to behaviorally disordered adolescents. The Pointer, 31, 25-30.
Veldman, D. J., & Sheffield, J. R. (1979). The scaling of sociometric nominations. Educational
and Psychological Measurement, 39, 99-106.
Verduyn, C. M., Lord, W., & Forrest, G. C. (1990). Social skills training in schools: An evaluation
study. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 3-16.
Vosk, B., Forehand, R. Parker, J. B., & Rickard, J. (1982). A multimethod comparison of popular
and unpopular children. Dccvelopmental Psychology, 18, 571-575.
Wahler, R. G., & Dumas, J. E. (1986). "A chip off the old block": Some interpersonal character-
istics of coercive children across generations. In P. S. Strain, M. J. Guralnick, & H. M. Walker
(Eds.), Children's social behac"ior: Dccve/opment, assessment, and modification (pp. 49-
91). New York: Academic Press.
REFERENCES 195
Wahler, R. G., & Fox, J. J. (1982). Setting events in applied behavior analysis: Toward a concep-
tual and methodological expansion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 327-338.
Wahler, R. G., Hughey, J. B., & Gordon, J. S. (1981). Chronic patterns of mother-child coercion:
Some differences between insular and non insular families. Analysis and inter-<.!ention in de-
velopmental disabilities, 1, 145-156.
Walker, H. M., Colvin, G. R., & Ramsey, E. R. (1995). Antisocial behavior in school settings. Pa-
cific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Walker, H. M., & Hops, H. (1976). Increasing academic achievement by reinforcing direct acade-
mic and/or facilitating nonacademic responses. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68,
218-225.
Walker, H. M., McConnell, S., Holmes, D., Todis, B., Walker, J., & Golden, N. (1988). The Walker
Social Skills Curriculum: The ACCEPTS program. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Walker, H. M.. & McConnell, S. R. (1995). The Walker-McConnell Scales of Social Competence
and School Adjustment. San Diego: Singular Publishing.
Walker, H. M., & Severson, H. R. (1991). Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders. Long-
mont, CO: Sopris West.
Walker, H. M., Steiber, S., & Eisert, D. (1991). Teacher ratings of adolescent social skills: Psy-
chometric characteristics and factorial replicability. School Psychology ReView, 20, 301-
314.
Walker, H. M., Todis, B., Holmes, D., & Horton, G. (1988). The Walker social skills curriculum:
The ACCESS program (adolescent curriculum for communication and effective social
skills). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Whalen, C. K., Henker, B., Collins, B. E., & Granger, D. (1987). Peer perceptions of hyperactivity
and medication effects. Child Development, 58, 816-828.
Wiggins, J. S. (1981). Clinical and statistical prediction: Where are we and where do we go from
here? Clinical Psychology ReView, 1, 3-18.
Wing, J. K. (1978). Schizophrenia: Towards a new synthesis. New York: Academic Press.
Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. (1966). Behavior therapy techniques. New York: Pergamon.
Worthen, B. R., Borg, W. R., & White, K. R. (1993). Measurement and evaluation in the schools:
A practical guide. White Plains, NY: Longman.
Young, K. R., & West, R. P. (1984). Parent training: Social skills manual. Logan, UT: Utah State
University, Department of Special Education.
Zaragoza, N., Vaughn, S., & Mcintosh, R. (1991). Social skills interventions and children with be-
havior problems: A review. Behavioral Disorders, 16, 260-275.
This page intentionally left blank