Thoughts On 'Yep' and 'Nope
Thoughts On 'Yep' and 'Nope
Thoughts On 'Yep' and 'Nope
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Duke University Press and The American Dialect Society are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access
to American Speech.
http://www.jstor.org
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
91
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
92
AMERICAN SPEECH
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
93
giving vent to his own snarl; with the oo constellation imitatively, the
speakermimickingthe appearanceof someoneelse.
2. Gesturemay be intonationsl. The effectshere are more subtle, inasmuch as fundamentalpitch is not markedlyaffectedby positions within
the range of normal human gestures.If we include, however, the alterations of timbre which can be detected and taken as symbolic of the
gestures and gestural attitudes that cause them, then clear-cut effects
may be noted, particularlythose brought about by changesof head position. I have tried the following test: Asking a group of observers(five
students of phonetics) to face away from me, I uttered, first with head
thrust forward(as one does when anxious or surprised),then with head
drawn back (as when indignant or annoyed), the question 'He's coming,
isn't he?' The fundamental pitch of the profile was kept as nearly the
same as could be managed. The fIve unanimously identified the timbre
producedby the forwardthrust as indicative of anxiety-surprise,and that
of the drawn-backposition as indignation-annoyance,these two alternatives having been suggested to them beforehand. It is likely that the
drawn-backposition may have forced a slight lowering of pitch in addition to other changes.
On the whole, the phenomena of gesture and tone are parallel rather
than interacting.Terminal downmotion of pitch is usually accompanied
by mouth-closing;terminal upmotion, above all in questions, often with
mouth remaining open. In very emphatic speech there is a tendency to
punctuatesentencestressby downwardjabs of the head (considerstressas
tonal or as syntactic,as you like). Most remarkableof all, I note a tendency
to make head movements,where they are any, conformto the melodic line
-upward movementas the tone goes up, downwardmovementas the tone
goes down. The best way to test this is to try to reverse it on certain
profiles:with 'I know,' uttered with I at high pitch and with downmotion
followed by terminal upmotion on know, let the head firstmove up on I
with a down- and up-motion following as know is pronounced; this is
easy to do, but the opposite-down-up-down-calls for conscious effort.
The same difficultyis experiencedin forcing a down-upmovement to accompany'I know' when uttered with I at high pitch and know with sharp
downmotion ending at low pitch-the attempt seems to result in some
involuntaryupmotion at the beginning of know. Reversalof this tendency
occasionallycomes by way of some stereotypedgesture,as with the deferential bow that may accompanyprincipal stressin the question 'Do you
know?'where know describesa hump-shapedtonal curve. Or the stereotyped gesturemay itself symbolizethe same thing that the tone symbolizes,
whence it becomes indiSerent whether the melodic line is imitated or
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
AMERICAN SPEECH
94
not; this happens with tonal profilesof admonition and the admonitory
gesture of sidewise motion of the head coming to rest at one side, with
the speaker peering out of the corner of his eye-a gesture which may
accompanythe utteranceor precede it.
3. Gesturemay be syntactic.As a determinantof questions, gesture
attains an importancethat it does not have in any other communicative
act whose chief ingredients are 'language.'There are various clues to
questions:verbal,such as interrogativewordsand do-didprefixes(referred
to by the etymological name of 'inversions');tonal, such as, primarily,
terminalupmotion; and contextual. The question is a complex,however,
of which gestureformsa necessarypart, since many utterances,without it,
are indistinguishableas questions.Take the utterance'Then you did it,'
with sharp upmotion and stress on you, and rapid downmotion concluding with pitch below singing level reached on it. With head erect,
lip-closureat end, and eyes averted, this is a statement. With head forward, mouth slightly open at end of utterance,eyebrowsraised and eyes
focused on the interlocutor'sface, this is a question. The only clues to its
questionnessare the gesturalconcomitants.Their importancehas not been
overlooked by the fiction-writers,who usually summarize them as an
'inquiring look.'
Why have gesturesseemedunattractiveas a branchof inquiry essential
to linguistics?There are probablyseveralreasons.First,'gesture'summons
to the mindsof mostpersonsnot facial contortionsbut bodilypostures.Says
Bloomfield:5'Still other responsesare visible, but not directly important;
they do not change the lay-out of things, but they do, along with speech,
serse as stimuli to the hearer.These actionsare facial expression,mimicry,
tone of voice (in so far as it is not prescribedby the conventions of the
language), insignificant handling of objects (such as fiddling with a
rubber band), and, above all, gesture.'This implies, and furtherreading
reveals, that gesture here is thought of as manual. It also implies that
facial gestures, unlike intonation, are never 'prescribedby the conventions of the language,'i.e., are accidentalconcomitants,lacking in stereotypes and bearing no constant and organizablerelationship to speech.
Facial movementsfrom this point of view are of a kind with chattering
teeth (fear and cold), stopped nasal passages(catarrh),chuckling (uncontrolled mirth),gasping(breathlessness),etc.-at best having a sort of nonce
expressiveness,at worst interfering with the more serious business of
*
communlcatlon.
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
gS
Most refractoryof all, however, is probably the fact that in facial gesture, as in intonation, it is utterly impossible to draw the line between
symbol (language)and symptom (non-language).The voice moves up at
the end of a question. Is this a learned, conventionalized,linguistic fact,
a symbolof questionness,or is it the result of nervoustension accompanying the feeling of uncertainty,which causes the musclesof the larynx to
contractand produce a rising pitch? It is, of course,both; but to anyone
who is wedded to the theory that linguistic phenomenamust not be contaminated by anything which is not preciselyanalogous to the ordinary
morphemes,long since freed of their instinctive ties, such a mixture of
symboland symptommust be avoided at all costs.The 'word'is comfortable to work with, for, being completely stylized, its form can be treated
without reference to its meaning. As with intonation, so with facial
gesture:leaving the mouth open at the end of an utterancecan be learned
as a device to suggestthat the argumentis not over; it is also an aultomatic
result of the intention, or thought, of continuing. A snarl is instinctive;
it is also the most potent device of incisive speech, and as such can be
learned.
The gestures referredto here are not viewed, as M. H. Krouts views
them, as 'autistic.'They are, on the contrary,to a large degreelearnedand
conventionalized,and hence are organizableand classifiable.They form
as much a part of our communicativesystem as words and tones, and
must, along with other communicativeacts,be integratedinto our organon
of that systembefore we can fully know how much importanceto attach
to any one of the parts-in particular,whether the present all-pervasive
attention to phonology is justified.7
1935,no. 4, pp. 1-126.
Monogrowphs}
6. Psychological
see R. H. Stetson,
7. Fora criticismof the currentfondnessfor 'logisticphonemics,'
Basesof Phonology,OberlinCollege,1945.
This content downloaded from 128.243.46.132 on Tue, 07 Apr 2015 16:09:16 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions