Sleep Predictors and Sleep Patterns - Carskadon
Sleep Predictors and Sleep Patterns - Carskadon
Sleep Predictors and Sleep Patterns - Carskadon
Original article
See Editorial p. 97
Abstract
Purpose: To characterize sleep patterns and predictors of poor sleep quality in a large population of
college students. This study extends the 2006 National Sleep Foundation examination of sleep in early
adolescence by examining sleep in older adolescents.
Method: One thousand one hundred twenty-five students aged 17 to 24 years from an urban
Midwestern university completed a cross-sectional online survey about sleep habits that included
the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, the Horne-Ostberg MorningnessEveningness Scale, the Profile of Mood States, the Subjective Units of Distress Scale, and
questions about academic performance, physical health, and psychoactive drug use.
Results: Students reported disturbed sleep; over 60% were categorized as poor-quality sleepers by
the PSQI, bedtimes and risetimes were delayed during weekends, and students reported frequently
taking prescription, over the counter, and recreational psychoactive drugs to alter sleep/wakefulness.
Students classified as poor-quality sleepers reported significantly more problems with physical and
psychological health than did good-quality sleepers. Students overwhelmingly stated that emotional
and academic stress negatively impacted sleep. Multiple regression analyses revealed that tension
and stress accounted for 24% of the variance in the PSQI score, whereas exercise, alcohol and caffeine
consumption, and consistency of sleep schedule were not significant predictors of sleep quality.
Conclusions: These results demonstrate that insufficient sleep and irregular sleepwake patterns,
which have been extensively documented in younger adolescents, are also present at alarming levels
in the college student population. Given the close relationships between sleep quality and physical and
mental health, intervention programs for sleep disturbance in this population should be considered.
! 2010 Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
1054-139X/10/$ see front matter ! 2010 Society for Adolescent Medicine. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.016
125
126
1:44
1:27
Bed Time
12:45
12:22
12:14
12:20
12:11
12:25
11:53
12:03
11:02
10:32
10:15
HS 10
HS 11
HS 12
College
Fresh.
College
Soph.
Year in School
10:26
10:09
10:06
9:49
8:08
6:28
HS 9
6:23
HS 10
10:08
9:51
9:52
Rise Time
9:54
week
weekend
10:51
HS 9
127
6:23
HS 11
7:59
7:59
week
weekend
6:31
HS 12
8:03
College
Fresh.
College
Soph.
Year in School
Figure 1. Bedtimes and rise times by year in school. Data from the high
school bins are taken from the 2006 Sleep in America Poll (n 1,602) [9].
128
Table 1
Prevalence of sleep disturbances as measured by the PSQI
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index
Bedtime
Sleep latency
Risetime
Mean, SD
How often have you had trouble sleeping
because.
Cannot get to sleep within 30 minutes
Wake up in middle of night or early morning
Wake up to use the bathroom
Cough or snore loudly
Cannot breathe comfortably
Feel too cold
Feel too hot
Have bad dreams
Have pain
Other reasons
How often have you.
Taken medicine to aid in sleep?
Had trouble staying awake during social
activities?
Had a problem getting the enthusiasm to get
things done?
Rate overall sleep.
26.2%
44.2%
56.1%
87.5%
80.6%
71.0%
33.5%
70.2%
73.1%
49.4%
41.4%
34.2%
32.7%
9.6%
13.3%
23.4%
42.8%
21.7%
19.2%
17.3%
21.5%
16.0%
7.4%
2.1%
4.7%
4.5%
19.3%
6.5%
5.4%
22.0%
10.9%
5.6%
3.8%
0.7%
1.4%
1.1%
4.5%
1.6%
2.3%
11.0%
82.1%
75.7%
11.0%
20.8%
4.0%
3.3%
2.9%
20.0%
19.8%
30.1%
33.0%
17.2%
Very good
11.0%
Global PSQI
Fairly good
55.0%
Optimal(15)
34.1%
Fairly bad
30.0%
Borderline(67)
27.7%
Very bad
3.9%
Poor(#8)
38.2%
129
Table 2
Sample characteristics by class and gender
N
ESS week
Class
Post hoc
1>4
*Gender
ESS weekend
Class
*Gender
Total PSQI
Class
Gender
SUDS week
*Class
1 < 2,3,4
*Gender
SUDS weekend
*Class
1<3
*Gender
Mean
SD
1
2
3
4
262
270
244
205
7.3
7.1
6.7
6.2
3.6
3.9
3.7
3.7
M
F
359
625
6.2
7.2
3.6
3.8
1
2
3
4
M
F
262
270
244
205
359
625
6.7
6.8
6.6
6.4
6.0
7.0
3.5
3.7
3.8
3.5
3.6
3.6
1
2
3
4
M
F
254
254
236
202
349
600
6.7
6.9
7.3
7.1
6.7
7.2
3.0
3.1
3.3
3.1
3.1
3.2
1
2
3
4
M
F
248
244
234
196
345
580
55.1
62.3
63.0
62.2
53.6
64.7
25.1
22.3
21.7
24.5
26.0
21.1
1
2
3
4
M
F
248
243
234
195
345
578
34.9
40.2
40.9
39.1
31.8
43.0
23.0
23.1
22.7
24.1
23.4
22.3
Morningness/eveningness
Class
Gender
Caffeinated drinks/weekday
*Class
1<3
Gender
Alcoholic drinks/weekday
*Class
*Gender
Mean
SD
1
2
3
4
273
279
255
213
49.3
50.4
49.4
50.3
9.0
9.3
9.9
10.1
M
F
378
645
49.6
50.0
10.0
9.4
1
2
3
4
M
F
305
312
271
232
420
705
0.7
0.7
0.9
1.0
0.8
0.8
1.0
1.1
1.1
1.2
1.2
1.0
1
2
3
4
M
F
305
312
271
232
420
705
0.9
1.0
1.2
1.1
1.0
1.1
1.0
1.3
1.6
1.6
1.3
1.5
1
2
3
4
M
F
305
312
271
232
420
705
0.2
0.2
0.5
0.5
0.5
0.3
0.6
0.6
0.9
0.8
1.0
0.5
1
2
3
4
M
F
305
312
271
232
420
705
2.8
2.5
3.0
3.2
3.9
2.2
4.2
3.7
4.0
4.0
5.1
3.0
Difference from total N reflects omissions in survey reporting. Asterisks indicate significant differences between class or gender. Bonferroni tested significant
differences (a .01) between classes are provided in the left side of the columns.
SUDS Subjective Units of Distress Scale; PSQI Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; ESS Epworth Sleepiness Scale.
130
Table 3
Differences in behavior by PSQI group
Sleep schedule
Total sleep time (h)
Bedtime, weekday
Risetime, weekday
Bedtime, weekend
Risetime, weekend
Bedtime Delay (h)
Weekend Oversleep (h)
Morningness/Eveningness
(df)
2,952
2,948
2,948
2,946
2,947
2,946
2,947
2,949
(df),N
6,948
6,947
(df)
2,897
2,897
2,897
2,897
2,897
2,897
2,916
2,916
(df)
!2
h
Post hoc
Optimal
Borderline
Poor
102.8
9.5
2.8
5.5
3.0
0.8
4.0
33.1
<.001
<.001
0.062
0.004
0.060
0.448
0.019
<.001
.01
.02
<.01
.01
<.01
<.01
<.01
.07
O > B>P
O,B < P
7.61
12:07 a.m.
8:06 a.m.
1:34 a.m.
10:03 a.m.
1.46
1.95
53
7.08
12:13 a.m.
7:54 a.m.
1:42 a.m.
10:03 a.m.
1.48
2.7
50
6.47
12:28 a.m.
8:06 a.m.
1:52 a.m.
10:16 a.m.
1.38
2.2
47
c2
Optimal
Borderline
Poor
<.001
<.001
25%
12%
32%
16%
46%
28%
Post hoc
Optimal
Borderline
Poor
O < B<P
O < B<P
O < B<P
O < B<P
O < B<P
O > B>P
O < B<P
O < B<P
7.48
8.6
7.01
9.44
8.29
14.29
49.9
30.8
9
9.56
8.76
12.09
9.96
13.38
59.9
38
10.61
10.31
10.66
14.92
11.82
12.09
70.7
46.6
Post hoc
Optimal
Borderline
Poor
46.4
30.8
!2
h
66.8
32.2
71.2
146.2
81.1
28.4
72.4
37.7
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
<.001
!2
h
0.59
0.03
<.01
<.01
2,952
2,952
0.53
3.42
.13
.07
.14
.25
.16
.06
.14
.08
O<P
O > B>P
1.0
1.07
0.990
1.24
1.08
1.35
c2
Optimal
Borderline
Poor
23.3
118
14.0
0.003
<.001
<.001
12%
4%
5%
22%
13%
2%
26%
33%
10%
(df)
Post hoc
Optimal
Borderline
Poor
2,917
2,915
(df),N
6,951
12,948
10,950
42.2
16.7
c2
39.2
39
29.3
<.001
<.001
p
<.001
<.001
<.001
O < B<P
O < B<P
5.32
5.58
Optimal
9%
4%
16%
6.95
6.83
Borderline
12%
3%
18%
8.08
7.34
Poor
21%
12%
22%
(df),N
Use OTC/Rx meds to wake >13/month
Use OTC/Rx meds to sleep >13/month
Use alcohol to get to sleep
6,871
6,949
2,681
h
!2
.04
.10
O optimal (PSQI >6), B borderline (PSQI 67), P poor (PSQI >7). a .01.
PSQI [ Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; OTC over the counter.
131
Table 4
Stepwise multiple regression predicting PSQI scores
F(5,877) 882, p < .001
Std. Error
(Constant)
Predictors, R2 29%
POMS-Tension
SUDS
MES
POMS-Depression
POMS-Anger
Nonsignificant variables
Age
Sex
Ethnicity
Caffeine/day
Alcohol/day
Bedtime delay
Weekend oversleep
GPA
Hrs Exercise/week
Hrs TV/Video/week
POMS-Confusion
7.449
3.453
.118
.024
$.060
.112
.102
.041
.005
.014
.036
.038
.080
.152
.115
$.103
.105
$.178
.031
$.214
$.065
$.018
$.010
.071
.210
.288
.083
.067
.104
.082
.250
.071
.055
.046
Beta
Sig.
2.158
.031
.143
.182
$.187
.149
.119
2.857
5.278
$4.290
3.088
2.669
.004
<.001
<.001
.002
.008
.034
.024
.012
$.038
.052
$.054
.013
$.027
$.028
$.010
$.009
1.125
.724
.398
$1.247
1.561
$1.707
.377
$.858
$.906
$.323
$.222
.261
.469
.691
.213
.119
.088
.706
.391
.365
.747
.825
GPA grade-point average; PSQI [ Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; SUDS Subjective Units of Distress Scale; MES Morningness Eveningness Scale;
POMS Profile of Mood States.
Recommendations
These results highlight a growing need for professionals to
focus on the quality as well as the quantity of sleep when
promoting mental and physical health in adolescents and
young adults. College students who are consistently getting
poor-quality sleep are at risk for problems far more serious
than simply struggling to function in daily activities. As
chronic insomnia is a risk factor for major mood [39] and
substance abuse disorders [40], physicians, college healthcare professionals, and residence life workers should be
more proactive in screening for sleep difficulties and in articulating the importance of sufficient, restorative sleep in
college students well-being.
Disclosure Statement
The authors have indicated no financial conflicts of
interest.
References
[1] Leger D, Massuel MA, Metlaine A, SISYPHE Study Group. Professional correlates of insomnia. Sleep 2006;29:1718.
[2] Leger D, Scheuermaier K, Phillip P, et al. SF-36: Evaluation of quality
of life in severe and mild insomniacs compared with good sleepers.
Psychosom Med 2001;63:4955.
[3] Katz DA, McHorney CA. The relationship between insomnia and
health-related quality of life in patients with chronic illness. J Fam Pract
2002;51:22935.
[4] Roth T, Jaeger S, Jin R, et al. Sleep problems, comorbid mental disorders, and role functioning in the national comorbidity survey replication. Biol Psychiatry 2006;60:136471.
132
[20] Roberts RE, Roberts CR, Duong HT. Chronic insomnia and its negative
consequences for health and functioning of adolescents: a 12-month
prospective study. J Adolesc Health 2008;42:294302.
[21] Medeiros ALD, Mendes DBF, Lima PF, et al. The relationships
between sleepwake cycle and academic performance in medical
students. Bio Rhythm Res 2001;32:26370.
[22] Tsai LL, Li SP. Sleep patterns in college students: gender and grade
differences. J Psychosom Res 2004;56:2317.
[23] Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, Monk TH, et al. The Pittsburgh Sleep
Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research.
Psychiatry Res 1989;28:193213.
[24] Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness
Scale. Sleep 1992;15:37681.
[25] Horne JA, Otsberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine
morningnesseveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976;4:97110.
[26] Wolpe J. The Practice of Behavioral Therapy, 3rd ed. New York:
Pergamon Press, 1982.
[27] McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Profile of mood states. EdITS
Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: EdITS/Educational and Industrial Testing Service, 1992.
[28] Roehrs T, Shore E, Papineau K, et al. A two-week sleep extension in
sleepy normals. Sleep 1996;19:57682.
[29] Lund HG, Whiting AB, Prichard JR. The relationship between sleep
quality, mood, and stress in college Women. Sleep Abstr 2006;29.
[30] Ohayon MM, Roth T. Place of chronic insomnia in the course of
depressive and anxiety disorders. J Psychiatr Res 2003;37:915.
a,c,*
,
Christine Acebo
b,c
, Mary A. Carskadon
b,c
a
Department of Psychology, Brown University, Providence, RI 02906, USA
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown Medical School, Providence, RI 02906, USA
E.P Bradley Hospital Sleep and Chronobiology Research Laboratory, East Providence, RI 02915, USA
b
c
Received 11 August 2006; received in revised form 14 November 2006; accepted 4 December 2006
Available online 26 March 2007
Abstract
Sleep/wake timing shifts later in young humans during the second decade of life. In this review we describe sleep/wake patterns,
changes in these patterns across adolescence, and evidence for the role of environmental, psychosocial, and biological factors underlying these changes. A two-process model incorporating circadian (Process C) and sleep/wake homeostatic (Process S) components
is outlined. This model may help us to understand how developmental changes translate to shifted sleep/wake patterns. Delayed
sleep phase syndrome (DSPS), which has a typical onset during the second decade of life, may be an extreme manifestation of
homeostatic and circadian changes in adolescence. We describe symptoms, prevalence, and possible etiology of DSPS, as well as
treatment approaches in adolescents.
2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Keywords: Adolescent; Development; Circadian rhythms; Sleep patterns; DSPS; Delayed sleep phase syndrome
1. Introduction
This review describes changes to sleep/wake behavior
during adolescent development and the contribution of
the circadian and sleep/wake homeostatic systems to
this changing behavior. We also review delayed sleep
phase syndrome (DSPS), which may be an extreme manifestation of these changes or may be a distinct clinical
entity. Last, we describe approaches to treat DSPS in
adolescents. For the purpose of this review, we consider
biological adolescence to span the second decade of life.
2. Developmental changes in sleep/wake (light/dark)
patterns
Sleep/wake patterns of developing adolescents are
often described in the context of a school year and have
*
been described separately for school (weekday) and nonschool (weekend) days. Sleep timing is often quite dierent during school vacations. Describing sleep patterns
during both school and vacation times provides a more
comprehensive account of developmental sleep/wake
behavior changes, yet few studies have examined vacation sleep patterns.
Table 1 summarizes adolescent self-reported sleep
patterns derived from survey studies that reported weekend and weekday data during the school year. These
data show that adolescents report going to bed later as
they get older. Studies from such countries as Canada
[1], Poland [2], Belgium [3], Australia [4], Finland [5],
and Brazil [6] show similar trends. Investigators associate this age-related change in bedtime on school nights
with a number of environmental factors, including
reduced parental inuence on bedtimes [7,8], increased
homework [9], and extra-curricular activities, such as
sports, musical groups, clubs, and service groups [10],
or part-time work [7,10,11]. Other environmental, usually stimulating activities, that often aect bedtime
603
Table 1
Mean self-reported school-year weekend and weekday sleep patterns from survey reports in the U.S
Weekday Bedtimea
Weekend Bedtimea
Weekend Risetimea
2130 (2140)
2124
2145 (2150)
2152
2137
2206 (2228)
2153
2205
2222 (2250)
2231
2255 (2305)
2305
2311
2320 (2342)
2326
2354
0705 (0700)
0642
0655 (0705)
0635
0610
0650 (0700)
0636
0559
0825 (0745)
0853
0835 (0835)
0912
0847
0845 (0845)
0921
0922
2238
2215
2246
2210
2224
2220
2307
2232
2244
2238
2252
2237
2313
2251
2258
2251
2324
2302
2359
2353
0017
2357
0011
0006
0042
0003
0041
2356
0028
0030
0115
0025
0039
0049
0124
0045
0628
0620
0556 (0615)
0605 (0627)
0600
0954
0940
0914 (0912)
0924 (0925)
0940
0623
0555
0608
0613 (0638)
0605
0952
0951
0915
0921 (0937)
0946
0623
0626 (0645)
0610
1006
0921 (0927)
0932
0631
0951
Reference
age/school grade
(2216)
(2243)
(2308)
(2314)
(0006)
(0027)
(0044)
(0051)
Values followed by parentheses indicate that authors computed means for boys and girls separately in the original report; the rst value = mean
time reported by girls; parenthetical value = mean time reported by boys.
604
ment provide some insight into these sleep/wake behavioral changes. We provide a general background of both
systems and then discuss how these processes appear to
change during adolescent development.
3. The circadian timing system
The circadian timing system provides temporal organization for regulatory mechanisms to facilitate adaptive
behavior, such as feeding, reproduction, and sleep/wake
cycles [23]. These coordinated temporal patterns, or circadian rhythms, are self-sustained and oscillate with a
period of about 24 h. The internal mechanism (pacemaker) that organizes these rhythms in mammals has
been localized to a small paired nucleus in the hypothalamus, the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN; [24]). Biological events or markers associated with these rhythms can
be used to estimate circadian time or phase.
Melatonin is a hormone secreted by the human pineal
gland that oscillates with a circadian rhythm. Levels of
the hormone are nearly absent during the daytime, rise
in the evening near ones usual bedtime, stay relatively
constant during the nighttime, and decline near ones
habitual wake-up time. Melatonin is suppressed by light
[25], and recent studies show that even room light levels
(200300 lux) can have a suppressive eect on human
endogenous melatonin production [2628]. The onset
of melatonin secretion, also called the dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) phase, is a marker of the circadian
timing system [29] and can be measured from saliva
samples collected at 30-min intervals in dim light
(<30 lux) [30]. Research laboratories often dene
DLMO phase as the time at which melatonin concentration rises above a designated threshold (e.g., 4 pg/mL
for saliva and 10 pg/mL for plasma melatonin). The
decline of melatonin, also called the dim light melatonin
oset (DLMO) phase, and the midpoint between
DLMO and DLMO are other phase markers of the circadian timing system derived from the melatonin
rhythm. Other rhythms such as core body temperature
have also been used to mark the circadian system; however, DLMO phase is currently thought to be the most
reliable marker of phase [31,32].
The circadian timing system oscillates with an intrinsic period slightly dierent from 24 h but synchronizes
(entrains) to the 24-h day in response to external timegivers, or zeitgebers. The primary synchronizing stimulus for the circadian timing system is the daily variation
of daylight and darkness [33]. The circadian system is
sensitive to light, especially during the nighttime, which
for humans is the usual sleep period. A phase response
curve (PRC) describes how light input is able to shift circadian rhythms earlier or later in time. Fig. 1 shows a
PRC to light for adult humans, constructed by Khalsa
and colleagues (2003) [34]. These researchers measured
circadian phase by way of plasma melatonin before
605
Fig. 1. Figure and gure legend are reprinted with permission from
Khalsa et al. (2003) [34]. Phase advances (positive values) and delays
(negative values) are plotted against the timing of the centre of the light
exposure relative to the melatonin midpoint on the pre-stimulus
constant routine (dened to be 22 h), with the core body temperature
minimum assumed to occur 2 h later at 0 h. Data points from circadian
phases 68 are double plotted. The lled circles represent data from
plasma melatonin, and the open circle represents data from salivary
melatonin in subject 18K8 from whom blood samples were not
acquired. The solid curve is a dual harmonic function tted through all
of the data points. The horizontal dashed line represents the
anticipated 0.54 h average delay drift of the pacemaker between the
pre- and post-stimulus phase assessments.
606
Upper Threshold
(Sleep Initiation)
Lower Threshold
(Wake Initiation)
WAKE
SLEEP
00:00 08:00
Time
WAKE
SLEEP
00:00 08:00
of nocturnal sleep in humans. As sleep pressure dissipates across the night, sleep is protected by the circadian
timing system, which achieves maximal levels of sleep
propensity during the second half of the habitual sleep
period.
Our laboratory has used the two-process model as a
framework to study adolescent sleep/wake regulation
[50,51]. We review studies that have investigated pubertal changes of the circadian and homeostatic sleep systems. These data may provide us with additional
insight into the observed sleep/wake behavioral changes
of adolescents.
5. Circadian timing and sleep/wake homeostasis during
adolescent development
5.1. Process C
The circadian timing system undergoes developmental changes during adolescence. Although sleep/wake
patterns have long been known to delay in adolescents,
behavioral factors (e.g., social and scholastic obligations) were assumed to be entirely responsible. The
notion that circadian timing may change was noted by
Carskadon and colleagues in a study of circadian phase
preference in young adolescents [52]. Phase preference,
or morningness/eveningness is a behavioral construct
related to the time of day best suited for waking behavior. Carskadon and colleagues found that phase preference was correlated with self-assessed pubertal
development, especially in girls [52]. Subsequent studies
conrmed that more mature and older adolescents prefer later timing of activities than younger, less mature
adolescents [22,53]. Adolescents with evening and morning phase preference also show dierences in patterns of
for the older group, and recent work shows that less
total sleep time may attenuate the phase shifting
response [59,60]. In other words, if adolescents are studied on their natural schedules, it is possible that older
adolescent may show a weaker response to light compared to younger adolescents. Dampening the response
to the phase advance portion of the light PRC may hinder synchronization.
Additional research is needed to understand
responses to light during adolescent development. Furthermore, given that the circadian timing system is most
sensitive to short-wavelength light (460 nm) [61,62],
further investigation of sensitivity to dened light bandwidths may help us understand how the circadian timing
system and its sensitivity to external zeitgebers may or
may not change during adolescence.
A developmental delay of circadian phase during
adolescence may also be related to a lengthening of
the intrinsic period of the circadian clock, that is, a
longer internal day length. In a preliminary analysis,
Carskadon and colleagues measured period in 27 adolescents aged 915 years, showing an average of
24.27 h [63]. The sample size was not adequate to test
dierences across Tanner stage; however, intrinsic period was signicantly longer than the average in adults,
as measured by others [51,6365]. Reports show that
entrained circadian phase in young adults was positively
associated with circadian period; a later circadian phase
was related to a longer period [66,67]. Furthermore,
recent work showed that a longer intrinsic period was
associated with a circadian timing system that more
readily phase delayed in constant conditions [68]. Preliminary data from adolescents and drawing parallels in
the adult literature leaves open the potential that a
longer period during adolescence could lead to a developmental circadian delay. This hypothesis still needs to
be tested with more participants across pubertal
development.
5.2. Process S
Recent modeling work of Jenni and colleagues [69]
provides data to support developmental changes to Process S. Using model simulation techniques of SWA,
results showed a slower accumulation of Process S during wake in mature adolescents (Tanner 5) compared to
pre- or early pubertal adolescents (Tanner 1 and 2)
under conditions of sleep deprivation. Furthermore,
the upper threshold (asymptote) for Tanner 5 participants was higher compared to Tanner 1 and 2 participants; the dierence between the thresholds was larger
for the Tanner 5 youngsters compared to Tanner 1
and 2. Further support for the slowing of Process S
comes from sleep latency data, which shows Tanner 1
participants falling asleep faster in the evening (2230
and 0030 test points) compared to Tanner 5 participants
607
608
aect the timing or duration of sleep; (2) sleep disruption that leads to insomnia and/or excessive daytime
sleepiness; and (3) impaired social, occupational, or
other spheres of functioning related to the sleep disturbance. The ICSD notes that objective ndings typically
show a delay demonstrated by diary and actigraphy of
sleep onset until 01000600 h, with wake-up time in late
morning or early afternoon, especially on weekends and
vacations; normal sleep from all-night polysomnography on the delayed schedule and prolonged sleep latency
on a conventional schedule; measures of circadian phase
(e.g., temperature recording or DLMO) that indicate a
phase delay; scores on a morningness/eveningness
(phase preference) questionnaire in the denite evening
type range. Dierential diagnosis issues include the
importance of distinguishing DSPS, particularly in adolescents, from normal sleep patterns where a delayed
schedule is maintained without distress or impaired
functioning. Social and behavioral factors, such as late
evening activities, school avoidance, and social or family
dysfunction, may have a role in the development and
maintenance of a syndromic delay. In addition, dierentiation of DSPS from other insomnias rests on the nding that sleep initiation and maintenance are normal
when the patient is allowed to sleep on his or her preferred schedule. Finally, the excessive daytime sleepiness
in these patients occurs on mornings on which they must
arise earlier than preferred; sleepiness abates if the preferred sleep schedule is allowed.
Prevalence estimates of DSPS range widely. The 1997
edition of the ICSD [78], estimated that 510% of
patients with insomnia complaints seen in sleep clinics
have DSPS. The 2005 ICSD-Revised [79] places this gure at 10%. Other reports based on questionnaires, telephone sampling, or mixed subjective and objective
measures estimate the prevalence of DSPS in the general
population anywhere between 0.13% and 3.1% [80]. A
recent study, based on interviews structured to question
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
Fourth edition (DSM-IV) criteria in 1124 European
adolescents aged 1518 years, reported a relatively low
rate (0.4%) of adolescents with some circadian disorder
[81]. The rate was 0.2% in a companion sample of young
adults, ages 1924 years. The authors noted that a larger
number of adolescents reported some indicators of circadian rhythm disorders, such as dierences between
real and desired sleep schedule, extra sleep on days o,
and diculties getting up in the morning, although they
did not report negative daytime consequences requisite
for DSPS classication. Furthermore, this study was
completed in Europe, where later school start times
(08000830 h) than in many areas of the United States
may better t the normally delaying patterns of adolescence and involve less distress related to poor sleep. The
prevalence rate for at least one symptom of insomnia in
the adolescent sample was 30%, and the most commonly
609
References
[1] Laberge L, Petit D, Simard C, Vitaro F, Tremblay R. Development of sleep patterns in early adolescence. J Sleep Res
2001;10:5967.
[2] Szymczak JT, Jasinska M, Pawlak E, Zwierzykowska M.
Annual and weekly changes in the sleepwake rhythm of school
children. Sleep 1993;16(5):4335.
[3] Van den Bulck J. Television viewing, computer game playing,
and Internet use and self-reported time to bed and time out of
bed in secondary-school children. Sleep 2004;27(1):1014.
[4] Henschel A, Lack L. Do many adolescents sleep poorly or just
too late? Sleep Res 1987;16:354.
[5] Saarenpaa-Heikkila OA, Rintahaka PJ, Laippala PJ, Koivikko
MJ. Sleep habits and disorders in Finnish schoolchildren. J Sleep
Res 1995;4(3):17382.
[6] Andrade MM, Benedito-Silva AA, Domenice S, Arnhold IJ,
Menna-Barreto L. Sleep characteristics of adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Adolesc Health 1993;14(5):4016.
[7] Carskadon MA. Patterns of sleep and sleepiness in adolescents.
Pediatrician 1990;17(1):512.
[8] Sleep in America Poll. Washington, DC, <http://www.sleepfoundation.org/>; 2006.
[9] Manber R, Pardee RE, Bootzin RR, Kuo T, Rider AM, Rider
SP, et al. Changing sleep patterns in adolescents. Sleep Res
1995;24:106.
[10] Carskadon MA. Adolescent sleepiness: increased risk in a high
risk population. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving 19891990;5/6(4/
1):317328.
[11] Carskadon MA, Mancuso J, Rosekind MR. Impact of part-time
employment on adolescent sleep patterns. Sleep Res 1989;18:114.
[12] OBrien EM, Mindell JA. Sleep and risk-taking behavior in
adolescents. Behav Sleep Med 2005;3(3):11333.
[13] Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA. Sleep schedules and daytime
functioning in adolescents. Child Dev 1998;69:87587.
[14] Carskadon MA, Davis SS. Sleepwake patterns in the highschool-to college transition: preliminary data. Sleep Res
1989;18:113.
610
[15] Link SC, Ancoli-Israel S. Sleep and the teenager. Sleep Res
1995;24a:184.
[16] Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Seifer R, Fallone G,
Labyak SE, et al. Evidence for the validity of a sleep habits
survey for adolescents. Sleep 2003;26(2):2134.
[17] White L, Hahn PM, Mitler MM. Sleep questionnaire in
adolescents. Sleep Res 1980;9:108.
[18] Strauch I, Meier B. Sleep need in adolescents: a longitudinal
approach. Sleep 1988;11(4):37886.
[19] Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M. Life between clocks:
daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms
2003;18(1):8090.
[20] Hansen M, Janssen I, Schi A, Zee PC, Dubocovich ML. The
impact of school daily schedule on adolescent sleep. Pediatrics
2005;115(6):155561.
[21] Crowley SJ, Acebo C, Fallone G, Carskadon MA. Estimating
dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) phase in adolescents using
summer or school-year sleep/wake schedules. Sleep
2006;29(12):163241.
[22] Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Pramstaller PP, Ricken J, Havel M,
Guth A, et al. A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr Biol
2004;14(24):R10389.
[23] Moore RY. The organization of the human circadian timing
system. Prog Brain Res 1992;93:10117.
[24] Moore RY. Circadian rhythms: basic neurobiology and clinical
applications. Annu Rev Med 1997;48:25366.
[25] Lewy AJ, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK, Newsome DA, Markey SP.
Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science
1980;210(4475):12679.
[26] Zeitzer JM, Dijk DJ, Kronauer R, Brown E, Czeisler C.
Sensitivity of the human circadian pacemaker to nocturnal light:
melatonin phase resetting and suppression. J Physiol
2000;526(Pt. 3):695702.
[27] Boivin DB, James FO. Phase-dependent eects of room light
exposure in a 5-h advance of the sleepwake cycle: implications
for jet lag. J Biol Rhythms 2002;17(3):26676.
[28] Smith KA, Schoen MW, Czeisler CA. Adaptation of human
pineal melatonin suppression by recent photic history. J Clin
Endocrinol Metab 2004;89(7):36104.
[29] Lewy AJ, Sack RL. The dim light melatonin onset as a marker
for circadian phase position. Chronobiol Int 1989;6(1):93102.
[30] Lewy A, Cutler NL, Sack RL. The endogenous melatonin prole
as a marker for circadian phase position. J Biol Rhythms
1999;14(3):22736.
[31] Klerman EB, Gershengorn HB, Duy JF, Kronauer RE.
Comparisons of the variability of three markers of the human
circadian pacemaker. J Biol Rhythms 2002;17(2):18193.
[32] Benloucif S, Guico MJ, Reid KJ, Wolfe LF, LHermiteBaleriaux M, Zee PC. Stability of melatonin and temperature
as circadian phase markers and their relation to sleep times in
humans. J Biol Rhythms 2005;20(2):17888.
[33] Czeisler CA, Richardson GS, Zimmerman JC, Moore-Ede MC,
Weitzman ED. Entrainment of human circadian rhythms by
lightdark cycles: a reassessment. Photochem Photobiol
1981;34:23947.
[34] Khalsa SB, Jewett ME, Cajochen C, Czeisler CA. A phase
response curve to single bright light pulses in human subjects. J
Physiol 2003;549(Pt 3):94552.
[35] Shanahan TL, Czeisler CA. Light exposure induces equivalent
phase shifts of the endogenous circadian rhythms of circulating
plasma melatonin and core body temperature in men. J Clin
Endocrinol Metab 1991;73(2):22735.
[36] Minors DS, Waterhouse JM, Wirz-Justice A. A human phase
response curve to light. Neurosci Lett 1991;133:3640.
[37] Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Arnedt J. Failure to identify
pubertally-mediated melatonin sensitivity to light in adolescents.
Sleep (Supplement) 2002;25:A191.
[82]
[83]
[84]
[85]
[86]
[87]
[88]
[89]
[90]
[91]
[92]
[93]
[94]
[95]
[96]
[97]
[98]
[99]
[100]
611
612
[101]
[102]
[103]
[104]
[105]
[106]
[107]
[108]
[109]
Sleep Medicine
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/sleep
Review Article
Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review
Neralie Cain, Michael Gradisar *
School of Psychology, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA, Australia
a r t i c l e
i n f o
Article history:
Received 3 December 2009
Received in revised form 15 February 2010
Accepted 19 February 2010
Available online xxxx
Keywords:
Child
Adolescent
Sleep
Media
Technology
Television
Computer
a b s t r a c t
Electronic media have often been considered to have a negative impact on the sleep of children and adolescents, but there are no comprehensive reviews of research in this area. The present study identified 36
papers that have investigated the relationship between sleep and electronic media in school-aged children and adolescents, including television viewing, use of computers, electronic gaming, and/or the internet, mobile telephones, and music. Many variables have been investigated across these studies, although
delayed bedtime and shorter total sleep time have been found to be most consistently related to media
use. A model of the mechanisms by which media use may affect sleep is presented and discussed as a
vehicle for future research.
! 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
ages or pubertal stages [8]. This suggests that environmental factors (such as decreased parental monitoring) and psychosocial factors (such as increased use of electronic media) have a
considerable influence on the amount of sleep obtained by adolescents [8]. Nevertheless, despite popular press [811] and the common practice for clinicians to emphasise the negative impact of
electronic media on sleep, there have been no comprehensive reviews of empirical research in this area.1 The current papers aims
are to (1) describe the evolution of technology and how this may
be impacting child and adolescent sleep, (2) review relevant research
to date, and (3) provide future directions for both research and clinical practice.
1389-9457/$ - see front matter ! 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
Socio-Economic Status?
Increased
Daytime
Media Use
Electronic
Media Device
in Bedroom
Age?
Possible Mechanism???
Increased PreBedtime
Media Use
Sleep
Problems
Impaired
Daytime
Functioning
Parental Control?
Fig. 1. A graphical representation of the potential impact of electronic media on sleep.
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
vision had a negative effect on their childs sleep [18]. These figures
are important, considering that much of the research reviewed in
this paper relies on parent-report data (see Section 7 for further
discussion of this issue).
In terms of television viewing in the evening or at bedtime, it
has been reported that as many as 82% of American adolescents
watch television after 9 pm and 34% watch DVDs or videos [20].
Watching television in the evenings has been associated with significantly shorter total sleep time on both weekends and weekdays
[33,34] and with a generally higher frequency of sleep problems
compared with children not watching television after 9 pm [30].
Similarly, children and adolescents who often or occasionally
watch television in the evening have significantly later bedtimes
and wake-up times on weekdays and/or weekend days [17,34].
Television viewing at bedtime has also been found to be significantly correlated with sleepwake transition disorders, disorders
of excessive somnolence, and overall sleep problem severity in
children aged 56 years [31]. A discrepancy of more than 1 h between weekday and weekend bedtimes has also been associated
with watching television before bedtime [17]. Subjective reports
from adolescents are also consistent with these findings [35].
The results presented in Table 1 reveal that many different variables have been investigated in relation to television viewing and
sleep among school-aged children. As yet, however, there is little
consensus regarding which aspects of sleep may be related to television viewing. The most consistent results to date seem to be decreased total sleep time, prolonged sleep onset latency, and
delayed bedtime. More research is clearly needed to determine
whether or not other aspects of sleep are also related to television
viewing.
It is important to note that all of the abovementioned studies
are correlational and that empirical evidence in this area is rare.
In a small experimental study, 11 boys (aged 1214 years) were
exposed to 1 h of a subjectively exciting movie, approximately 2
3 h prior to bedtime [36]. Their sleep was then monitored using
both subjective and objective measures (i.e., sleep diary and polysomnography, respectively). Compared to a control evening when
they did not watch television or play video games at all, participants had significantly lower sleep efficiency (i.e., percentage
of time in bed that the individual was actually asleep) on the
Table 1
Relationship between television viewing and sleep variables.
Authors
Age (years)
516
1116
510
410
1718
917
69
610
1115
1619
1317
1217
1215
613
1415
1213
56
511
511
1219
612
TIB
TST
SOL
WASO
GTB
WUT
DS/T
BR
SA
Parasomnias
SWT
DES
X
U
X
U
U
U
U
U
U
X
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
X
U
X
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
Note: U = examined in study (significant relationship between variables); X = examined in study (non-significant); TIB = shorter time in bed; TST = shorter total sleep time;
SOL = longer sleep onset latency; WASO = more night waking; GTB = delayed bedtime; WUT = delayed wake-up time; DS/T = daytime sleepiness or tiredness; BR = bedtime
resistance; SA = sleep anxiety; SDB = sleep-disordered breathing; SWT = sleepwake transition disorders; DES = disorders of excessive somnolence.
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
any sleep disorders (e.g., bedtime resistance, sleep anxiety, parasomnias) [14] and, in contrast to another study [19], found no significant relationship with wake-up times on weekdays or weekend
days nor with weekday bedtimes or daytime sleepiness.
When considering the use of computers or electronic games in
the evening or at night it has been reported that 55% of American
adolescents access the internet and 24% play computer games after
9 pm [20]. Playing video games or using a computer before bed has
been associated with later bedtimes [17,37], shorter total sleep
duration [17,33,37], later wake-up time on weekend days [17], increased daytime tiredness [37], and poorer overall sleep quality
[45]. A discrepancy of more than 1 h between weekday and weekend bedtimes and a discrepancy of more than 2 h between weekday and weekend wake-up times have also been associated with
playing video games before bed [17].
In terms of internet use, three studies have obtained consistent
results despite measuring internet use in different ways. Van den
Bulck measured the amount of time that adolescents spent using
the internet at any time of day [19], Oka and colleagues categorized
children according to whether or not they used the internet before
bed two or more times per week [17], while Yen and colleagues
used a self-report questionnaire to assess symptoms of internet
addiction [46]. Taken together, this research revealed that internet
use is related to delayed bedtimes [17,19], delayed weekend wakeup times or out-of-bed times [17,19], shorter total sleep times
[17,46], shorter time in bed on weekdays [19], higher levels of
tiredness [19], and higher levels of subjective insomnia [46] (see
Table 3). A discrepancy of more than 2 h between weekday and
weekend wake-up times was also associated with using the internet before bed [17].
In a small experimental study (described earlier), participants
were exposed to 1 h of video-game play approximately 23 h prior
to bedtime [36]. Compared to a control evening, participants had
significantly longer sleep onset latencies on the experimental night
and experienced significant changes in their sleep architecture,
with less time spent in slow-wave sleep. But there was no difference in overall sleep efficiency or time spent awake after sleep onset. A second experimental study involved 22 male participants
aged 1215 years who participated in three experimental conditions: playing a violent video game, playing a non-violent video
game, neither playing video games nor watching exciting television programs [47]. Games were played for 2 h, ending half an hour
before bedtime, and a comprehensive questionnaire and sleep
diary was also completed on each occasion. There were no significant differences between the two game-playing conditions for any
sleep item. Compared to the control night, however, participants
went to bed significantly later on both gaming nights and, after
playing the non-violent game, participants reported that it was significantly easier to fall asleep, and their out-of-bed time the following morning was significantly earlier. Weaver and colleagues
conducted a similar experimental study involving 13 male adolescents (aged 1418 years) and found that, compared to a passive
DVD viewing control condition, pre-sleep video game playing resulted in a small increase in sleep onset latency and a small decrease in subjective sleepiness [48]. But in contrast to previous
studies, there were no changes in sleep architecture.
Table 2
Relationship between sleep variables and use of television as a sleep aid.
Authors
Age (years)
TST
SOL
WASO
410
1217
U
U
GTB
U
BR
SA
Parasomnias
Daytime sleepiness/tiredness
X
U
Note: U = examined in study (significant relationship between variables); X = examined in study (non-significant); TST = shorter total sleep time; SOL = longer sleep onset
latency; WASO = more night awakenings; GTB = delayed bedtime; BR = bedtime resistance; SA = sleep anxiety.
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
Age (years)
TIB
1217
1215
613
1217
1415
1213
511
1518
1218
511
612
1218
TST
SOL
GTB
WUT
BR
SA
Parasomnias
Daytime sleepiness/tiredness
U
U
U
U
U
U
X
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
Note: U = examined in study (significant relationship between variables); X = examined in study (non-significant); TIB = less time in bed; TST = shorter total sleep time;
SOL = longer sleep onset latency; GTB = delayed bedtime; WUT = later weekend wake-up time; BR = bedtime resistance; SA = sleep anxiety.
Table 4
Relationship between sleep variables and mobile telephone use.
Authors
Age (years)
TST
1315
1217
1215
1218
1217
1519
1218
SOL
WUT
Evening
type
Dissatisfied with
sleep
Nap
frequently
Daytime sleepiness/
tiredness
Subjective
insomnia
U
X
U
U
U
X
X
X
Note: U = examined in study (significant relationship between variables); X = examined in study (non-significant); TST = shorter total sleep time; SOL = longer sleep onset
latency; WUT = later weekend wake-up time.
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
activities a mobile phone provides and its portability this could become the most frequently used media device (day and night) and
thus should be an essential item assessed in future studies.
7. Study limitations
The research studies reviewed here are plagued by a number of
limitations. The first limitation is that only three experimental
studies were found [36,47,52]. Instead, most report on cross-sectional correlational studies, which means that causal direction is
difficult to ascertain. It is possible that children and adolescents
who use technology in the evening do so because they do not need
as much sleep as their peers or because they already have a delayed sleep pattern and cannot fall asleep at their designated bedtime. For example, in a household where a parent encourages his/
her teenager to go to bed at 10 pm, the delayed adolescent may go
to his/her bedroom (but not feel sleepy) and occupy time by using
his/her mobile telephone, computer or television until feeling sleepy enough to go to bed several hours later. Another factor, such as
parenting style or the presence of childhood behavioural problems,
may also impact both media use and sleep problems among younger children. For example, a child who exhibits difficult behaviours
may be encouraged to watch television to give the parents a break
and keep the child occupied. Consistent with this hypothesis, research has revealed that sleep problems are common among children with behaviour problems [53] and some studies have found
that children with behavioural and emotional problems spend
more time watching television [54].
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
8. Possible mechanisms
A number of mechanisms have been proposed by which media
use might impact sleep quality or quantity. First, media use may
directly displace sleep or other activities related to good sleep hygiene (such as physical activity). Second, media use in the evenings
may cause children to become physiologically aroused, making it
more difficult for them to relax prior to bedtime. A number of studies have found increases in physiological arousal associated with
playing computer games [47,5962], and with the advent of more
physical computer gaming technology (such as the Nintendo Wii)
this issue becomes increasingly more relevant. Third, evening
exposure to bright light from television or computer screens may
suppress melatonin and consequently delay the circadian rhythm
[63]. Finally, electromagnetic radiation from mobile telephones
has been found to change sleep architecture [64,65] and delay melatonin production [66].
It is beyond the scope of this paper to comprehensively review
the support for and against the model presented in Fig. 1. Furthermore, it is acknowledged that much of the research in this area has
been correlational in nature and, consequently, the causal directions presented in the proposed model are largely speculative. In
addition to the results relating to the potential mechanisms described above, however, studies providing preliminary support
for other aspects of the model have already been mentioned in this
review. For example, the percentage of children who watch television or play video games before bed has been found to be higher
among children who have a television or video game console in
their bedroom [17] and the relationship between age and weekend
total sleep time has been found to be mediated by time spent using
a computer [26]. It would be useful for future studies to comprehensively test this model, using research designs that move beyond
the correlational analyses which are prevalent in this area.
9. Conclusions
Despite the aforementioned limitations, it appears that the use
of electronic media by children and adolescents does have a negative impact on their sleep, although the precise effects and mechanisms remain unclear. Across different media types, the most
consistent results have been obtained regarding delayed bedtime
and shorter total sleep time associated with excessive media use.
In future research, it would be good to develop and test a model
of the mechanisms by which media use affects sleep (such as that
presented in Fig. 1). This would provide further evidence to inform
clinicians, parents, young people, and the popular press and will
likely become even more relevant as technology continues to develop in the future.
Looking towards the future, the research community faces the
challenge of keeping up with advances in technology. For example,
studies on mobile telephones have so far only considered making
and receiving calls and sending and receiving text messages. Mobile telephone technology, on the other hand, has evolved at such
a rate that many people now use their mobile telephone to access
the internet, send and receive emails, engage in social networking,
listen to music, and play games. Furthermore, as television and
computer screens become increasingly larger and bright light
exposure from screens is considered to influence melatonin production, future studies may need to incorporate variables which
would not have been considered in previous research.
Eventually, guidelines about the duration and timing of electronic media use should be developed for children and adolescents
of different ages and these should be made known to the public.
Considering the evidence to date it appears that watching television and using other media devices at bedtime should generally
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
[21]
[22]
[23]
[24]
[25]
[26]
[27]
[28]
[29]
[30]
[31]
[32]
[33]
[34]
[35]
[36]
[37]
[38]
[39]
[40]
[41]
[42]
[43]
[44]
Please cite this article in press as: Cain N, Gradisar M. Electronic media use and sleep in school-aged children and adolescents: A review. Sleep Med (2010),
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2010.02.006
0920
CIRCADIAN PHASE PIIEIERENCE, SLEEP PATTEINS AND
PERCEIVED HEALTI{ IN ADOLESCENTS
Snlith Ll 't, Boncl TLt :, RalJa' TI , Shcu ke' Iil,lt -t, Carskacbn ll'[A] :
rDeparturent of Psychiatry ancl Hurnan Behavror, Alperr Medical
School of Brorvn University, Providence, RI, United States,:Sleep
Research Laboratory, Bradley Hospital, Providence, RI. United States.
rDepartrnent of Medicine, Alper-t Medical School of Brorvn University,
Plovrdence, RI, United States, rDeparnent of Psychology. University
of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
inadeqr.rate sleep
have been associated rvith clinical and fnctional outcolnes. The impact
ofcircadian pliase prelerence on sleep patterns in adolescents in relation
to perceived health and health outcoures is less u'ell explorecl. The prev
ent study exanlines associations among circadian phase prelerence, total
sleep tirne. and perceived health in a saurple ofadolescents.
Methods: 145 parricipants recruited from a large sarnple olstudents recently admitted to college (agcs l7 - 21, urean age oflS years;68 rnales)
completed an online questionnaire to assess sleep, rnood, and other behaviors. Circadian phase prelerence rvas rneasured using the Hornestberg Momingness-Eveningness Questionnaire (lvlEQ); parricipants
rvere categorized into three groups based on the original scoring criteria;
rnomin-q types (n:21), neLrtral types (n: 84). and evening types (n:
40). Perceived health was taken from a single itent ("Horv rvor.rld you
charactedze your general health?") on a scale of I to 5 (1 : "excellent,"
5 : "poor"). Sleep pattems on school nights and non-school nights over
the past two rveeks rvere assessed using iteurs frour ari adolescenl sleep
habits sun'ey.
Results: As a continuous variable, higher MEQ score (associated rvith
rronringness) was related to better perceived health (R! : .034, P < .05);
however; rvhen self-r'eported school night total sleep tirne rvas entered
into the regression equation, VEQ score rvas no longer a significant predictor of perceived health (ARr =.005, P >.1). Analyses of MEQ as
a categorical variable levealed that evening types reported a later bed
time on school nights (rnean: l2:36arn, SD = 1.07hrs) than rnoming
(mean = ll:34prn, SD:0.96hrs) and neutal types (rnean - ll:44pm,
SD = 0.86hrs), but wake tirnes did not difler arnong MEQ groups (mean
: 6:44arn, SD : 0.69hrs). Evening-types reporled less total sleep tirle
on school nights (mean - 6.79hrs. SD: 1.69) than morning (mean:
8.03hrs, SD: Li7) and neutral types (urean = 7.69hrs, SD = L34).
Conclusion: Adolescents rvho sell-identify as evening types sleep less
than morning types or neutral types on school nights due to later bedtines and sirnilar wake tirnes. Evening types perceive their health as
worse than their moming type counteqrafis; this darity is accounted
for by the difference in total sleep time on school nights betrveen the
groups.
Support (IfAny): This rvork rvas supporled by the National Institute for
Mental Health (grant R0l MH079179). Tilenn Raffray is supported by
the ELrropean Sleep Center, Paris Ftance and I'lnstitut Servier, Neuillysu-Seine, France.
4308