Abstract
The control of goal-directed, instrumental actions by primary motivational states, such as hunger and thirst, is mediated by two processes. The first is engaged by the Pavlovian association between contextual or discriminative stimuli and the outcome or reinforcer presented during instrumental training. Such stimuli exert a motivational influence on instrumental performance that depends upon the relevance of the associated outcome to the current motivational state of the agent. Moreover, the motivational effects of these stimuli operate in the absence of prior experience with the outcome under the relevant motivational state. The second, instrumental, process is mediated by knowledge of the contingency between the action and its outcome and controls the value assigned to this outcome. In contrast to the Pavlovian process, motivational states do not influence the instrumental process directly; rather, the agent has to learn about the value of an outcome in a given motivational state by exposure to it while in that state. This incentive learning is similar in certain respects to the acquisition of “cathexes” envisaged by Tolman (1949a, 1949b).
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
References
Adams, C. D., &Dickinson, A. (1981). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,33B, 109–122.
Asratyan, E. A. (1974). Conditioned reflex theory and motivational behavior.Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis,34, 15–31.
Baeyens, F., Eelen, P., van den Berg, H., &Crombez, G. (1990). Flavor-flavor and color-flavor conditioning in humans.Learning & Motivation,21, 434–455.
Balleine, B. (1992). Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends upon incentive learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,18, 236–250.
Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1991). Instrumental performance following reinforcer devaluation depends upon incentive learning.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,43B, 279–296.
Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1992). Signalling and incentive processes in instrumental reinforcer devaluation.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,45B, 285–301.
Berridge, K. C. (1991). Modulation of taste affect by hunger, caloric satiety, and sensory-specific satiety in the rat.Appetite,16, 103–120.
Berridge, K. C., Flynn, F. W., Schulkin, J., &Grill, H. J. (1984). Sodium depletion enhances salt palatability in rats.Behavioral Neuroscience 98, 652–660.
Berridge, K. C., Grill, H. J., &Norgren, R. (1981). Relation of consummatory responses and preabsorptive insulin release to palatability and learned taste aversions.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,95, 363–382.
Bindra, D. (1972). A unified account of classical conditioning and operant training. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds.),Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 453–482). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual motivational alternative to response-reinforcement.Behavior & Brain Sciences,1, 41–52.
Bolles, R. C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning.Psychological Review,95, 394–409.
Bolles, R. C. (1975).Theory of motivation. New York: Harper & Row.
Bolles, R. C., Holtz, R., Dunn, T., &Hill, W. (1980). Comparison of stimulus learning and response learning in a punishment situation.Learning & Motivation,11, 78–96.
Cabanac, M. (1971). Physiological role of pleasure.Science,173, 1103–1107.
Campbell, D. H., Capaldi, E. D., Sheffer, J. D., &Bradford, J. P. (1988). An examination of the relationship between expectancy learning and preference conditioning.Learning & Motivation,19, 162–182.
Capaldi, E. D., Davidson, T. L., &Myers, D. E. (1981). Resistance to satiation: Reinforcing effects of food and eating under satiation.Learning & Motivation,12, 171–195.
Capaldi, E. D., &Myers, D. E. (1982). Taste preferences as a function of food deprivation during original taste exposure.Animal Learning & Behavior,10, 211–219.
Capaldi, E. D., Myers, D. E., Campbell, D. H., &Sheffer, J. D. (1983). Conditioned flavor preferences based on hunger level during original flavor exposure.Animal Learning & Behavior,11, 107–115.
Capaldi, E. D., Sheffer, J., &Owens, J. (1991). Food deprivation and conditioned flavor preferences based on sweetened and unsweetened foods.Animal Learning & Behavior,19, 361–368.
Capaldi, E. D., Viveiros, D. M., &Davidson, T. L. (1981). Deprivation stimulus intensity and incentive factors in the control of instrumental performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,7, 140–149.
Colwill, R. C., &Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,11, 120–132.
Colwill, R. C., &Rescorla, R. A., (1986). Associative structures in instrumental learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 20, pp. 55–104). New York: Academic Press.
Crocetti, C. P. (1962). Drive level and response strength in the barpressing apparatus.Psychological Reports,10, 563–575.
Davidson, T. L. (1987). Learning about deprivational intensity stimuli.Behavioral Neuroscience,101, 198–208.
Davidson, T. L., Flynn, F. W., &Jarrard, L. E. (1992). Potency of food deprivation intensity cues as discriminative stimuli.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,18, 174–181.
Dickinson, A. (1980).Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dickinson, A. (1986). Re-examination of the role of the instrumental contingency in the sodium-appetite irrelevant incentive effect.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,38B, 161–172.
Dickinson, A. (1989). Expectancy theory in animal conditioning. In S. B. Klein & R. R. Mowrer (Eds.),Contemporary learning theories: Pavlovian conditioning and the status of traditional learning theory (pp. 279–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dickinson, A., &Balleine, B. (1990). Motivational control of instrumental performance following a shift from hunger to thirst.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,42B, 413–431.
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1987a). Pavlovian processes in the motivational control of instrumental performance.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,38B, 201–213.
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1987b). The role of the instrumental contingency in the motivational control of performance.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,39B, 77–93.
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1988). Motivational control of instrumental performance: The role of prior experience of the reinforcer.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,40B, 113–134.
Dickinson, A., &Dawson, G. R. (1989). Incentive learning and the motivational control of instrumental performance.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,41B, 99–112.
Dickinson, A., &Nicholas, D. J. (1983). Irrelevant incentive learning during instrumental conditioning: The role of drive-reinforcer and response-reinforcer relationships.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,35B, 249–263.
Eisman, E., Theios, J., &Linton, M. (1961). Habit strength as a function of drive in a bar pressing situation.Psychological Reports,9, 583–590.
Estes, W. K. (1943). Discriminative conditioning: 1. A discriminative property of conditioned anticipation.Journal of Experimental Psychology,32, 150–155.
Everitt, B. J., &Stacey, P. (1987). Studies of instrumental behavior with sexual reinforcement in male rats (Rattus norvegicus): 2. Effects of preoptic area lesions, castration, and testosterone.Journal of Comparative Psychology,101, 407–419.
Fitzsimons, J. T. (1961). Drinking by nephrectomized rats injected with various substances.Journal of Physiology,155, 563–579.
Garcia, J. (1989). Food for Tolman: Cognition and cathexis in concert. In T. Archer & L.-G. Nilsson (Eds.),Aversion, avoidance and anxiety (pp. 45–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gormezano, I., &Tait, R. W. (1976). The Pavlovian analysis of instrumental conditioning.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences,11, 37–55.
Grice, G. R., &Davis, J. D. (1957). Effect of irrelevant thirst motivation on a response learned with food reward.Journal of Experimental Psychology,53, 347–352.
Grill, H. J., &Berridge, K. C. (1985). Taste reactivity as a measure of the neural control of palatability. In J. M. Sprague & A. N. Epstein (Eds.),Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 1–61). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Grindley, G. C. (1932). The formation of a simple habit in guinea pigs.British Journal of Psychology,23, 127–147.
Heathers, G. L., &Arakelian, P. (1941). The relationship between strength of drive and rate of extinction of a bar-pressing reaction in the rat.Journal of General Psychology,24, 243–258.
Hendersen, R. W., &Graham, J. (1979). Avoidance of heat by rats: Effects of thermal context on the rapidity of extinction.Learning & Motivation,10, 351–363.
Hershberger, W. A. (1986). An approach through the looking-glass.Animal Learning & Behavior,14, 443–451.
Horenstein, B. R. (1951). Performance of conditioned responses as a function of strength of hunger drive.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,44, 210–224.
Hovancik, J. R. (1978). The effect of deprivation level during noncontingent pairings and instrumental learning on subsequent instrumental performance.Learning & Motivation,9, 1–15.
Hull, C. L. (1943).Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Irwin, F. W. (1971).Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. Philadelphia: Lippincott.
Jenkins, H. M., &Harrison, R. H. (1960). The effects of discrimination training on auditory generalization.Journal of Experimental Psychology,59, 246–253.
King, D. L. (1979).Conditioning: An image approach. New York: Gardner.
Koch, S., &Daniel, W. J. (1945). The effect of satiation on behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength.Journal of Experimental Psychology,35, 167–187.
Konorski, J. (1967).Integrative activity of the brain: An interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Krieckhaus, E. E., &Wolf, G. (1968). Acquisition of sodium by rats: Interaction of innate mechanisms and latent learning.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,65, 197–201.
Lopez, M., Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1992a). Incentive learning and the motivational control of instrumental performance by thirst.Animal Learning & Behavior,20, 322–328.
Lopez, M., Balleine, B., &Dickinson, A. (1992b). Incentive learning following reinforcer devaluation is not conditional upon motivational state during re-exposure.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,45B, 265–284.
Meachum, C. L. (1990). The role of response-contingent incentives in lithium chloride-mediated suppression of an operant response.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,42B, 175–195.
Miller, N. E. (1963). Some reflections on the law of effect produce a new alternative to drive reduction. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 65–112). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Miller, S., &Konorski, J. (1969). On a particular form of conditioned reflex.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,12, 187–189. [Translation by B. F. Skinner of: Sur une forme particulière des reflex conditionnels.Les Compte Rendus des Seances de la Société Polonaise de Biologie, 1928,49, 1155–1157].
Morgan, M. J. (1974). Resistance to satiation.Animal Behaviour,22, 449–466.
Morrison, S. D., Mackay, C., Hurlbrink, E., Wier, J. K., Nick, M. S., &Miller, F. K. (1967). The water exchange and polyuria of rats deprived of food.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology,52, 51–67.
Mowrer, O. H. (1960).Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.
Pavlov, I. P. (1927).Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pavlov, I. P. (1932). The reply of a physiologist to a psychologist.Psychological Review,39, 91–127.
Pelchat, M. L., Grill, H. J., Rozin, P., &Jacobs, J. (1983). Quality of acquired responses to taste byRattus norvegicus depends upon type of associated discomfort.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,97, 140–153.
Perin, C. T. (1942). Behavior potentiality as a joint function of the amount of training and the degree of hunger at the time of extinction.Journal of Experimental Psychology,30, 93–113.
Peterson, D., &McHose, J. H. (1980). The residual deprivation effect in instrumental conditioning as a function of deprivation level at the time of exposure to the reinforcer.Animal Learning & Behavior,8, 642–646.
Rescorla, R. A. (1992). Depression of an instrumental response by a single devaluation of its outcome.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,44B, 123–136.
Rescorla, R. A., &Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationship between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning.Psychological Review,74, 151–182.
Revusky, S. H. (1967). Hunger level during food consumption: Effects on subsequent preference.Psychonomic Science,7, 109–110.
Revusky, S. H. (1968). Effects of thirst level during consumption of flavored water on subsequent preference.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,66, 777–779.
Rolls, B. J., &McFarland, D. J. (1973). Hydration releases inhibition of feeding produced by intracranial angiotensin.Physiology & Behavior,11, 881–884.
Rozin, P., &Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust.Psychological Review,94, 23–41.
Sackett, R. S. (1939). The effect of strength of drive at the time of extinction upon resistance to extinction in rats.Journal of Comparative Psychology,27, 411–431.
Saltzman, I., &Koch, S. (1948). The effect of low intensities of hunger on the behavior mediated by a habit of maximum strength.Journal of Experimental Psychology,38, 347–370.
Sheffield, F. D. (1965). Relation between classical conditioning and instrumental learning. In W. F. Prokasy (Ed.),Classical conditioning: A symposium (pp. 302–322). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner, B. F. (1932). On the rate of formation of a conditioned reflex.Journal of General Psychology,7, 274–285.
Skinner, B. F. (1936). Conditioning and extinction and their relation to drive.Journal of General Psychology,14, 296–317.
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Sutton, R. S., &Barto, A. G. (1981). An adaptive network that constructs and uses an internal model of its world.Cognition & Brain Theory,4, 217–246.
Thorndike, E. L. (1911).Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: Macmillan.
Toates, F. M. (1978). A physiological control theory of hunger-thirst interaction. In D. A. Booth (Ed.),Hunger model (pp. 347–374). London: Academic Press.
Tolman, E. C. (1949a). The nature and function of wants.Psychological Review,56, 357–369.
Tolman, E. C. (1949b). There is more than one kind of learning.Psychological Review,56, 144–155.
Tolman, E. C. (1959). Principles of purposive behavior. In S. Koch (Ed.),Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2, pp. 92–157). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Trapold, M. A., &Overmier, J. B. (1972). The second learning process in instrumental learning. In A. H. Black & W. F. Prokasy (Eds),Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 427–452). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Van Hemel, P. E., &Myers, J. S. (1970). Control of food-motivated instrumental behavior in water-deprived rats by prior water and saline drinking.Learning & Motivation,1, 86–94.
Verplanck, W. S., &Hayes, J. R. (1953). Eating and drinking as a function of maintenance schedule.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,46, 327–333,
Webb, W. B. (1955). Drive stimuli as cues.Psychological Reports,1, 287–298.
Wetzel, R. J. (1959). The effect of experience with a taste reward.Journal of Comparative A. Physiological Psychology,52, 267–271.
Woods, S. C., Vasselli, J. R., &Milan, K. M. (1977). Iron appetite and latent learning in rats.Physiology & Behavior,19, 623–626.
Young, P. T. (1961).Motivation and emotion. New York: Wiley.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Additional information
This research was supported by the S.E.R.C. and the Commonwealth Scholarship Commission. We thank A. Watt and R. A. Boakes for their comments on a draft of the manuscript.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Dickinson, A., Balleine, B. Motivational control of goal-directed action. Animal Learning & Behavior 22, 1–18 (1994). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199951
-
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199951